PART TWO GERMANIC PHONOLOGY

Back to Table of Contents

8. Reconstruction

The established method of Indo-European comparative grammar has in a tentative way 'reconstructed' the most essential phonological features of prehistoric Indo-European. If in a given word all, or most, Indo-European languages show the same sound, we assume that this represents the original condition of Indo-European. For instance, the root consonants in the Greek words Gk. πατήρ, Gk. μήτηρ, δω(ρον), Gk. δόμος, are identical with those of many other Indo-European languages, for instance, L. mater, pater, do(num), domus, OSL. мать, да(нь), Sk. māt{U+0101}, pit{U+0101} d{U+0101}(nam), dámas, and we assume that they also occurred in the corresponding Indo-European words. But in the case of differences between the Indo-European languages, we take it for granted that variations of sounds are due to phonetic changes in some or all of them. Thus, the stem vowel of an important type of present tense forms is e, and the ending of the first person singular is ō in the earliest accessible forms of many Indo-European languages. The Indo-Iranian languages, however, have a, ā: Gk. φέρω, L. ferō: Sk. bhárā(mí). It was formerly assumed that Indo-Iranian in this respect represented the original condition of Indo-European. But since 1878 (34 a) it has been quite generally admitted that a, ā in these languages is a development from earlier e, ō. Accordingly, from these verb forms is now reconstructed an IE *bherō; the sign * implies that the form is not found in any document, but represents a reconstruction on a comparative basis. In principle, such reconstructions are merely tentative for mulae stating, for instance, that 'IE bh' is the sound that appears under ordinary conditions as bh in Indic, as φ in Greek, as f or b in Latin, and as b or ƀ in the other Indo-European languages; or that 'IE e represents Indo-Iranian a, but Greek, Latin, Celtic, Germanic e. Nevertheless, in most cases the reconstruction implies the phonetic character of Indo-European sounds with a considerable degree of probability. This is not even altered by the assumption that phonetic variations in different parts of the Indo-European territory may have existed at all times. It is, for instance, quite possible that in the extreme East, Indo-European ŏ, which became ă in Indo-Iranian, had a more open quality than in the rest of the territory. The sound that we represent by 'IE bh' may have varied since earliest times. Perhaps it was pronounced as a fortis in some districts, and as a lenis in others. The former articulation may have led to L. f, Gk. φ, the latter to Gmc., Celt. b ƀ, Sl. b. Even as mere formulae, these reconstructions are of the highest systematic value and have greatly contributed to the development of Indo-European comparative grammar into an exact science. But we should of course attempt, in selecting symbols for reconstructed sounds, to reach the closest possible approach to their actual phonetic character.

Back to Table of Contents

9. Phonetic Drift and Phonetic Law

The habitual ways of producing and combining speech sounds constitute a group of habits for every language, which is generally called its basis of articulation, or its organic or phonetic basis. It is felt as the dominant note of a language, impressing upon it its peculiar stamp: the clean-cut preciseness French, the contrasting ruggedness of North German, the self-constrained calmness of English, the 'insinuating grace' of Russian (Jespersen, Growth and Structure of the English Language 3). This can only be understood when it is realized that the individual habits of sound production and combination harmonize with one another in their typical characteristics to such an extent that they represent an acoustic unit-- one chord composed of many single elements. Within certain limits, this acoustic dominant of a language is a rather permanent quality pervading long periods of its life and governing its historical changes. The sum of these habits of articulation may be called the Phonetic Tendency or Phonetic Drift1 of the language. The Phonetic Basis is the static, descriptive aspect of the acoustic character of a language, the Phonetic Drift its dynamic, historical aspect.

Continuing through generations and centuries, the phonetic drift exerts either a stabilizing, or a modifying influence upon individual sounds. Thus, contrasting, strong accentuation tends to weaken the articulation of unaccented syllables; a general trend towards energetic articulation may increase the muscular tension and force of expiration in the production of certain sounds, changing, for instance, 'lenes' to 'fortes', pure stops to aspirates or spirants; in languages of relatively gentle articulation neighboring sounds are apt to be assimilated, while in those of harsher articulation they may tend to retain their original character. The isolated types of such sound changes are termed Phonetic Laws, and since 1876 (A. Leskien, Die Deklination im Slavisch-Litauischen und Germanischen) it must be considered one of the chief tenets of linguistic science, that in principle phonetic laws do not admit of exceptions. They are consequences of the phonetic drift of a given language at a given time, and like causes lead to like effects. In that sense phonetic laws are truly 'ausnahmslos' But the very concept of phonetic drift implies various articulations of the same type of speech sound-- the same 'phoneme' —under various conditions. For instance, in languages of the Germanic group, the phoneme t may be fortis or lenis, aspirated or unaspirated, and this may lead to apparent exceptions to phonetic laws, since the sound may develop differently under different conditions: In Middle Franconian, t, as a rule, develops into an affricate or a spirant (ts or ss), but in habitually unstressed words, such as et, dat, it remains unchanged (17 d). But such variations do not diminish the methodical value of the formulation and application of phonetic laws.

The drift of living languages can be ascertained by actual observation. The drift of dead or reconstructed languages can only be deduced by combining and comparing their phonetic laws as isolated consequences of the drift. But it must always be kept in mind that the drift is the primary factor, and that the phonetic laws are merely its component parts

H. Schuchardt, Ueber die Lautgesetze 36: 'Welchen Sinn haben alle die Tausende von Lautgesetzen, solange sie isoliert bleiben, solange sie nicht in höhere Ordnungen aufgelöst werden? ... Im Einzelnen müssen wir das Allgemeine finden lernen, und demnach ist aucb die Erkenntnis einer Tataacbe, welche das ganze Sprachleben beherrscht, von grösserer Wichtigkeit all die Erkenntnis irgendwelcher Erscheinungsformen.'

J. Vendryes, Mélanges linguistiques 116: 'Une loi phonétique ne peut être reconnue valable que si elle est d'accord avec les principes qui régissent le système articulatoire de la langue au moment où elle agit. ... Tout changement phonétique peut être considéré comme du a l'action de forces intimes et sécrètes, aux quelles convient assez bien le nom de tendances. Ce sont ces tendances qui modifient sans cesse la structure de la langue, et l'évolution de chaque idiome résulte en dernière analyse d'un jeu perpetuel de tendances. ... La notion de tendance phonétique est plus exacte theorétiquement, et pratiquement plus féconde que celle de loi phonétique. Elle seule permet de déterminer avec précision la cause des changements phonétiques et d'interpréter scientifiquement ceux mêmes qui paraissent les plus rebelles à toute discipline scientifique.'

CONSONANTISM

The Indo-European Consonants

Back to Table of Contents

10. Modes of Articulation

Back to Table of Contents

a

According to present standards of recon struction, Indo-European had very few genuine spirants. Only s and its assimilated form z are generally recognized (12). Brugmann assumed a spirant of undefined phonetic character, for which he used the signs þ ð. Probably this was really a palatal spirant (ç j or š ž), voiceless or voiced according to surroundings; cf. Brugmann, KG 301; Hirt, Idg. Gr. 1. 231.

Among the numerous stops, the following modes of articulation are assumed:

Back to Table of Contents

b

(1) The Pure Voiceless Stops, p t k pronounced without aspiration, as in modern Romance and Slavic languages:

Back to Table of Contents

c

(2) The Pure Voiced Stops, b d g; b was extremely rare, but d and g (the latter of several varieties, see 11) were frequent sounds:

Back to Table of Contents

d

(3) The Aspirated Voiceless Stops, ph th kh (= p t k in English, Scandinavian, and Standard German). Evidence for the existence of these sounds can be found only in Sanskrit and Greek:

Back to Table of Contents

e

(4) The Aspirated Voiced Stops (Sonant Aspirates, Mediae Aspi-ratae), bh dh gh. These appear as genuine aspirated stops (similar to the sound groups in English words like abhor, adhere, foghorn) only in the Indic (not Indo-Iranian) group of Indo-European languages, as φ θ χ in Greek, as f þ h in Primitive Italic (in Latin, f f h initially, b d h medially) as ƀ ð ȝ in Primitive Germanic (24), and as b d g, partly alternating with ƀ ð ȝ, in the other IE languages:

Back to Table of Contents

f

The correctness of the reconstructions bh dh gh (posited by Curtius, KZ 2. 321) and ph th kh is doubtful.1 Sounds of this type (also nh lh rh) are very frequent in the Tibeto-Burmese group of languages,2 which extends far into India, but they are rare elsewhere, and entirely lacking everywhere in Europe. They occur also in the Dravidian and Munda languages,3 which occupy a large part of central and southern India. It is generally assumed that Indic borrowed from the latter 'cerebral' (supra-dental, point-inverted) ṭ ḍ ḷ ṇ, and there is a strong probability that Indic bh dh gh were likewise substitutions for other Indo-European sounds, taken over from Tibeto-Burmese and Dravidian languages surrounding, or partly absorbed in, the Indo-European languages of India.4 Similarly, ph th kh may owe, if not their existence, at least their continuation and spread to the prevalence of these sounds in the native languages of India. They occur in Sanskrit chiefly in certain emphatic words and in borrowings from native languages, particularly in names of native animals and plants. Besides, it seems that p t k after s were inclined to become ph th kh,5 but there is also a small number of words in which these sounds occur under other conditions.6 It seems not unlikely that the voiceless aspirates are a remnant from a period when pure stops and aspirated voiceless stops formed one phoneme, that is, when the speaker, without being conscious of the difference, usedp t k under normal conditions and ph th kh under conditions of emphasis and in the neighborhood of certain sounds; this is more or less the case in present South German. Subsequently, most Indo-European languages gave preference to one or the other variety of the phoneme. Only im Greek and Sanskrit both varieties came to be felt as separate sounds. In Sanskrit this was probably due to the example of the native languages. In Greek, conditions are difficult to judge as long as the pronunciation of φ θ χ is not entirely certain. According to the present standard view, these 'letters' had in Classical Greek the articulation of aspirated voiceless stops, so that for Greek as well as for Sanskrit a double system of voiceless stops, pure and aspirated, would have to be assumed. In this, Greek would stand alone among European languages. The arguments for the standard view are weighty, but hardly quite convincing. The inscriptional spellings are ambiguous, and the phonetic arguments insufficient. At any rate, in Christian times Greek φ θ χ were surely pronounced as voiceless spirants, and they may have been that since Indo-European times.7

To reconcile the treatment of the sonant aspirates in Italic and Greek, it used to be assumed (since Ascoli, KZ 18. 417) that Italic and Greek formed one linguistic group for some time after the separation from the Indo-European stock, and that in this group bh dh gh became ph th kh, but nothing in the general phonetic character of these languages-- their phonetic basis and drift—- can be adduced to support this argument. On the whole, it seems that reconstructed IE bh dh gh are essentially a remnant from that period of linguistic science, when Sanskrit was considered the fundamental language of the Indo-European group. The claim that the Sanskrit vowel system was practically identical with Indo-European vocalism has been given up since 1878 (cf. 8), but the Indo-European consonant system as it appears in our standard grammars is still patterned on that of Sanskrit and differs greatly from the 'phonetic pattern' (Sapir, Language 57) of all other Indo-European languages.

Back to Table of Contents

g

If these sounds were not sonant aspirates, they almost must have been spirants. It is nearly unthinkable that voiced spirants became voiceless spirants in Italic (and possibly in Greek); therefore, only voiceless spirants are left, so that Italic would have preserved the original Indo-European articulation. It would have to be assumed that these spirants were 'lenes' rather than 'fortes' (like f in Ger. Ofen, rather than in offen); this would account for their inclination to become voiced in most IE languages; in fact, it is quite probable that these 'lenes' really were phonemes that could be either voiced or voiceless, perhaps according to emphasis or surrounding sounds. This would be comparable to the treatment of voiceless spirants in English, Norse, and some West Middle German dialects: Gmc. f þ between vowels were voiced since OE and ON times (27); f in words like Käfer, elf = [kɛvər,εləvə] is voiced, e.g. in Hessian. Our IE bh dh gh should therefore be considered phonemes that could appear either as voiceless lenes spirants, or as voiced spirants.

Back to Table of Contents

h

The following sections will show that we arrive at complete phonetic consistency, if we reconstruct the following Indo-European modes of articulation:

(1) p t k, the pure voiceless stops, and ph th kh, the aspirated voiceless stops, as transitory

variations of the same phonemes.

(2) b d g, the voiced stops.

(3) Voiceless spirants in lenis pronunciation. For these, the signs φ θ χ instead of bh dh gh were suggested by the author in Mod. Phii. 16. 102:

*φer, *φrāter, *neφos; *ruθəro-, *meθyo-, *χeu-, *weχ, *χeɐχ, *χosti-, *χorto-, *steiχ-, *χwermo-, *χwen-, *(s)neiχw-, instead of *bher-, *brātēr, *nebhos, *dhē-, *rudhəro-, *medhyo-, *ĝheim-, *ĝheu-, *weĝh-, *ĝheɐgh-, *ghosti-, *ghorto-, *steigh-, *ghwermo-, *ghwen-, *(s)neighw- {ILLEGIBLE NEED THE BOOK!}

Back to Table of Contents

i

Whoever prefers to consider Indo-European reconstructions as formulae pure and simple, without any phonetic connotation, may treat these symbols merely as a simplified way of writing bh dh gh; certainly, it is simpler to write *χwermo- than *ghwermo-, or, with Brugmann's symbols: *q*hermo-{CANNOT DECIPHER!}. If IE φ θ χ are accepted as the original sounds, we find the following: Certainly in Italic, and possibly in Greek, they remained voiceless spirants. Everywhere else they became voiced spirants. This is the condition that we find in primitive Germanic (18), and that we may safely assume for prehistoric Celtic, Slavic, Iranian, etc. In all of these languages, they became in time partly or entirely voiced stops (24). In Sanskrit, however, under the influence of native languages, voiced aspirates were substituted.

Detailed arguments for this view are given by Prokosch, Die indo-germanische "Media Aspirata", Mod. Phii. 15-18 (1918-20).8 However, for the sake of conformity with all standard grammars and etymological dictionaries, the established symbols bh dh gh are used in the present book.

Back to Table of Contents

11. Places of Articulation.

According to the place of contact in the mouth, we distinguish the following groups of Indo-European con sonants:

Back to Table of Contents

a

(1) The Labials, p b bh. Under ordinary circumstances, all three were bilabial (formed with both lips), but bh later became labio-dental f (formed by contact of lower lip and upper teeth) in Italic (medially, b in Latin), and, much later, also in Germanic, in those dialects and positions in which it remained a spirant: E. give, but Ger. geben < *ghebh-; cf. 24. In Greek, φ remained bilabial.

Back to Table of Contents

b

(2) The Dentals, t d dh. Whether these were pure dentals, as in French, or alveolars, as in German, or even post-alveolars, as in American English, can hardly be ascertained. In Italic, dh became f (medially, b or d in Latin: verbum, medius < *wṛdhom, *medhjos), see 10 e. The same transition is frequently noticed in individual English pronun ciations, like fimble for thimble; Russian has regularly f for θ in borrowings from Greek: Marfa, Feodor, fita for Μάρθα, Θεόδωρος, θητα

Back to Table of Contents

c

(3) The 'Gutturals'. This somewhat inept term is generally used in comparative grammars to designate consonants that are articulated either against the hard or the soft palate, and it may well serve as a collective term of expedience. The stops k g and the corresponding spirants generally, though by no means always, approach the articulation of neighboring sounds. Thus, k in king, car, cool, and g in give, garden, good, are articulated at the front, middle, or back palate respectively. Ger. ch is a pre-palatal spirant [ç], in ich, echt, but a velar (post-velar) spirant\[χ], in Dach, doch, Tuch; North German intervocalic g is palatal [j], in liegen, legen, but velar [ȝ], in lagen, bogen, trugen. Indo-European comparative grammar distinguishes Palatals (= Pre-Palatals), Velars, and Labio-Velars. The latter are back-palate sounds, which are accompanied by lip-rounding-- a frequently observed tendency of articulation in sounds that are pronounced by raising the back of the tongue, such as the typical u- and o-vowels. Authorities differ greatly as to the transcription of these sounds. The most widely accepted symbols are those used by Karl Brugmann (in his Grundriss der vergleichenden Grammatik der indogermanischen Sprachen, Kurze vergleichende Grammatik der indogermanischen Sprachen, etc.):

Palatals: k^, ĝ, ĝh -- *k^ṃtón, *ĝénos-, *ĝheim-

Velars: q q qh -- *qəp-, *steq-, *qhosti-

Labio-Velars: qw, qw, qwh -- *seqw, *qwem-, *qwhermo- {DIFFICULT TO RENDER THE LETTERS}

The letters k g gh are, in Brugmann's transcription, reserved for those cases where it is uncertain which articulation prevailed in a given Indo-European word, such as OSL. gąsъ, Lith. žąsís 'goose' = *qhəns- or *qhəns-?; Lith. akmuõ, Sk. aśman- = *aqmen- or *ak^men-?, Gk. ζυγόν, Sk. yugám = *juqom or juqwom? Cf. Brugmann, KG 233, Note 3 and 244, Note; Hirt. Idg. Gr. 1. 239 f.

For the purposes of Germanic grammar, the distinction between palatals and velars is immaterial (see below). Labio-velars, however, must be indicated as such whenever there is evidence of the existence of the labial element (lip-rounding). H. Hirt uses in his Handbuch des Urgermanischen k g gh for palatals and velars (in his Indogermanische Grammatik he uses k' g' g'h for palatals, and k g gh for velars), but kʷ gʷ ghʷ for labio-velars. Streitberg, UG and Loewe, GS use k^ k, ĝ g, ĝh gh gwh; Bethge, Altg. Dialekte, k^ k q, ĝ g q, ĝh gh qh. This book transcribes ordinarily palatals and velars by k g gh and labio-velars by kw gw ghw. But where Germanic must be compared with Eastern Indo-European languages, palatal articulation will be indicated by k^, ĝ, ĝh. This is especially necessary in those cases where Gmc. ɧw to does not go back to labio-velar kw (Brugmann's q*), but to a palatal followed by an independent labial spirant, as in Sk. aśva-, Go. aiƕa- 'horse', from IE *ek^wo-, not *ekwo- (*eqwo-). The transliteratikw is preferable to q or qw, because in Germanic as elsewhere, the labial element disappears under certain conditions (23), and the remaining velar does not differ in any way from a pure velar or a palatai. Cf. L. sequor—secutus, Go. saiƕan, OHG. sehan-- OE (Anglian) sǣgon: IE *sekw-; L. veniō, Go. qiman, OHG. queman, koman: IE *gwem-, gwṃ-; Gk. 'ομφή 'voice', Go. siggwan, OHG. singan.

Back to Table of Contents

d

Kentem and Satem. As stated in section 2, it is customary to distinguish two groups of Indo-European languages according to the treatment of the Indo-European palatals: they became sibilants in the eastern group, but appear as velars in the western group. But the labio-velars also exhibit a difference of articulation in the two groups: In kentum languages they are apt to preserve the labial element-- in fact, under certain conditions they even lose the velar articulation; in satem languages, the labial glide always disappears, so that there is no distinction between velars and labio-velars. Thus there is no individual IE language (except perhaps Albanian? Pedersen, KZ 36. 291 ff.) that possesses all three series of gutturals. The kentum languages have palato-velars and labio-velars. The satem languages have sibilants (or, partly, other spirants, such as Sk. h) and velars. The following table represents the fundamental developments in the two groups, omitting secondary changes, such as the Indie and Slavic palatalization of velars, Verner's Law in Germanic, etc.

{INSERT TABLE 1}

1 π β φ before α ο ω, τ δ θ before ε η ι, κ γ χ before and after υ; *kwel-/kwol-/kwl- 'turn': τελος 'goal' (turning-point), πόλος 'axis, pole', κύλος 'wheel' ('turn-turn')

k^: *k^ṃtón-- Sk. śatám, Av. satəm, OSL. sъto, Lith. šim˜tas-- Gk. <'εκατόν, L. centum

ĝ: *ĝen-, *ĝnō- 'know'-- Sk. jān-, OSL. znati, Lith. žinóti, Gk. Gk. γι-γνώσκω, L. co-gnō-scō

ĝh: ĝheim- 'winter, storm'-- Sk. hima, OSL. zima, Lith. žiema, Gk. χειμών, L. hiems

k: *kr(e)w-- Sk. kravíš- 'flesh', OSL. krъvъ, Gk. κρέας 'meat', L. cruor 'blood'

g: Sk. sthagáyati 'covers', OSL. o-stegъ 'dress', Gk. στέγω L. tegō 'cover'

gh: Sk. -stignōti 'climbs', OSL. stigną 'come', Gk. στείχω 'march', L. ve-stīgum 'track'

kw: Sk. kas 'who', OSL. kъ-to, Gk. πόθεν 'whence', L. quod 'what'

gw: Sk. gácchati 'goes', Gk. βαίνω 'go', L. veniō 'come'

ghw: Sk. ghnánti 'they kill', Lith. genù, OSL. ženą 'drive', Gk. θείνω 'kill', φόνος 'murder', L. of-fendō 'wound'.

In spite of this far-reaching and significant contrast in the treatment of 'gutturals', the assumption of these two branches of Indo-European languages merely on this phonological basis hardly seems justified. Phonetically, there need not be any fundamental difference between the change from IE *k^ṃtóm to Av. satəm and the change from L. centum to Fr. cent. There merely exists the formal difference that in the latter case we know that the transition was due to the influence of the following front vowel, while in the former case we assume that in Indo-European certain f- g-sounds had palatal articulation, regardless of their surroundings, and that in the eastern group they generally became sibilants. But it is entirely possible that the kentum-satem change was also essentially an assimilation, although there is little chance that we shall be able to reach back far enough into prehistoric times to prove it. The word for '100', for instance, had no vowel in the first syllable in the form that we can reconstruct. But it is more than probable that different case forms of this word originally had different vowel grades: *kṃt-, *kemt-, *komt-. In certain languages, then, the consonant belonging to the front-vowel form (k^) prevailed in all forms of the word, while in others the velar or back-velar articulation was carried through. Roughly speaking, the palatal articulation prevailed mainly in the eastern half of the Indo-European territory, but Albanian, a satem language, is farther west than Greek, while Tocharian, in the extreme east, is a kentum language. Phonetically, it is not at all surprising that languages which in an alternative gave preference to palatal, rather than velar, articulation, should also be inclined to give up the labial glide of the labio-velars, although this is rather frequent in the western group too (23). Therefore, only so much is safe to say, that the various 'guttural' sounds are inclined to be fronted in a number of Indo-European languages, and that in languages that have this tendency the labial glide disappears. In historical times, French and other Romance languages offer perfect parallels. On the one hand, kentum became [sā], and on the other, quod became [kə].

Back to Table of Contents

12. The Silbant S

Back to Table of Contents

a

The Silbant s was frequent in Indo-European: *seks, *septḿ '6, 7': Sk. şaş, saptá, L. sex, septem; *stā- 'stand': Sk. sthā-, L. stāre.

Before voiced consonants it became voiced: *ni-sd-o- > *nizdo- = L. nidus, E. nest; *o-sd-o = Gk. όζος = [ozdos], Ger. Ast.

In Greek, s disappeared between vowels, and became h or disappeared initially before vowels, semivowels, nasals, and liquids; in Latin, it was voiced, and then became r, between vowels ('Rhotacism') : Gk. έξ, 'επτά = L. sex, septem; Sk. janasas = Gk. *γένεσος > γένους, L. generis.

Back to Table of Contents

b

'Movable s'. Numerous words begin in some IE languages with s + consonant, and in others without s; sometimes the same variation occurs within the same language: L. taurus, Gk. ταυρος E. steer; Gk. στέγος, τέγος 'roof, L. tegō 'cover', toga 'covering, cloak', E. thatch; Go. skaidan 'separate', L. caedō 'cut'. The variation is not fully explained, but probably in many cases, s is the remnant of a prefix or preposition; in other cases, an original s- may have been lost in sentence connection (sandhi).

Back to Table of Contents

13. Sonorous Consonants

Back to Table of Contents

a

The 'liquids', l r, and the 'nasals', m n(ŋ) are consonants by articulation, since the air passage through the mouth is obstructed. The liquids are fundamentally spirants, the nasals are stops. But in their pronunciation the air passes through a comparatively large resonance chamber, in the mouth in the case of the liquids, and in the nasal cavity in the case of the nasals. This produces the acoustic effect of vowel resonance, so that these sounds may be said occupy a position between consonants and vowels. They may have vocalic (syllabic) function, as in E. middleman = [middlmæn], Ger. handeln, Czech prst, čtvrtku, vlk.

Syllabic function of these sounds (Brugmann'a liquida sonans and nasalis sonans) is in IE grammar usually indicated by a circle, less frequently a dot, below the letter: ḷ ṛ ṃ ṇ. Sanskritists generally use the dot, as in the name of the language, sãskrta. But it is hardly necessary to use any diacritic mark at all, as Czech spelling indicates. They represent in all cases the reduced form of diphthongs (36), and it is rather an expedient formula than an unequivocal fact, if our reconstruction assumes syllabic consonants rather than liquids or nasals accompanied by reduced vowels. The distinction is merely one between variations of the same phoneme, as in Ger. rechnete, handelte = [rεçntə, handltə] or [rəçnətə, handəltə]. With the exception of l r in Sanskrit, where both appear as , all IE languages show a vowel before them, more rarely after them. Sanskrit has ṛ a (am an); Greek, al ar, a (am an) ; Latin, ol or em en; Germanic, ul ur um un. Cf. 36, 39.

Back to Table of Contents

b

Back to Table of Contents

c

B. In vocalic function:

  • Back to Table of Contents

    14. The Semi-Vowels

    The Semi-Vowels, j w, are essentially vowels as to articulation, since the air passage is, in general, somewhat narrowed, but not suffiiently obstructed to characterize them as spirants. As reduced forms of diphthongs, they appear as pure vowels,i u (39); on the other hand, they tend to be narrowed to spirants or even stops, as in *jugóm 'yoke', Gk. ζυγόν = [dzugon]; Sk. dváyōs 'of two', tueggia; Gmc. *trewa-, tryggr (33 d). The bilabial semi-vowel (E. w) tends to become a labio-dental spirant, v, (cf. E. water: Ger. Wasser). To indicate this double character, the letters i̯ u̯ are frequently used, but there are advantages in the use, in reconstructed IE forms, of j w for true consonantic function, and of i u for pure vowels or diphthongal glides. (However, j v w are used in this book as everywhere, if historical spelling requires it.)

    THE GERMANIC CONSONANTS

    A. The Consonant Shift

    Back to Table of Contents

    15. Lautverschiebung

    Back to Table of Contents

    a

    'Sound Shift' was the term coined by Jacob Grimm in the second edition of his Deutsche Grammatik (1822) to designate a very large complex of interrelated phonetic changes through which the Germanic system of stops and spirants developed from the corresponding IE consonants. It is probably the most comprehensive group of sound changes that has been observed in the history of any language. But the individual changes are of such homogeneous character that finally the Germanic consonants constitute practically the same 'phonetic pattern' as the Indo-European consonants, especially if traditional bh dh gh are interpreted as voiceless spirants. Every single genuine consonant (cf. 13, 14) had altered its mode of articulation, but the fundamental types of the Indo-European and the Germanic consonants are the same.

    Back to Table of Contents

    b

    Rarely in German books, but rather frequently in English and French works, the whole complex of the consonant shift is termed 'Grimm's Law', 'loi de Grimm'. Recently protests have been voiced against that term (e.g. by Jespersen, Language 43, and by R. C. Boer, Oergermaansch Handboek 116). The substance of the facts has been summed up by Collitz, A Century of Grimm's Law, Language 2. 174 ff.:

    Grimm was by no means the first scholar to observe that in the Germanic languages consonants are often at variance with those of the cognate languages. Such discrepancies were especially noted and commented upon by students of Gothic from Franciscus Junius to the Swedish Professor Johannes ab Ihre and James Jamieson, the author of a well-known Etymological Dictionary of the Scottish language. Yet for a long time observations to this effect remained isolated, and amounted to little more than a mixture of truth and errors.

    Matters, however, took a different turn when, two years after the publication of Bopp's Conjugationssystem der Sanskritsprache, the great Danish philologist Rasmus Rask brought out his prize essay on the origin of the Icelandic language. Like Franz Bopp, Rask is one of the pioneers in the field of Indo-European philology. He began to investigate in a systematic manner the changes which the Germanic consonants had undergone in comparison with those of the cognate languages, and arrived at results that have proved — with few exceptions —reliable. His results are of great interest to us, the more so, as they have undoubtedly exercised considerable influence on Jacob Grimm's work. Grimm became acquainted with Rask's essay when he had nearly finished seeing the first edition of the first volume of his Grammar through the press. He hastened in the preface to that volume to voice his obligations to the author of the essay in the broadest and warmest possible manner. 'Meanwhile Rask's excellent prize-essay has furnished far-reaching information as to the many points of contact existing between the Germanic and the Lettic, Slavic, Greek, and Latin languages.' As far as the mere facts of the first Germanic shift are concerned, Rask, no doubt, was acquainted with most of the single paragraphs of Grimm's Law, and we may readily understand how the impression could obtain here and there that the law had actually been discovered by him. Nothing, however, could be a greater injustice to Grimm. Granted that Rask observed several consonant changes that play an important part in the shifting, we cannot possibly speak of Rask's Law in the singular....With Grimm the stress lies decidedly on the inner reason connecting the various parts of the shifting. He felt able to set forth a single law incorporating all its phases. His notion of such a law was based on the observation of a threefold uniformity:

    Back to Table of Contents

    c

    (1) the second or High German shifting proceeds in general on the same lines as the first or common Germanic shifting;

    (2) one and the same general formula is applicable to the various sets of consonants, whether they be labials or dentals or gutturals; (3) the shifting proves to imply a fixed sequence of the principal forms of the shifting, based on the arrangement of the three classes of consonants involved in the order of media, tenuis, aspirata....

    Such then is Grimm's Law: not merely a set of observations on consonant changes resulting from the first Germanic shifting, but rather a general theory as to the mutual relation between certain consonant changes, occurring as a rule in combination with each other. However much Grimm may be indebted for details to his predecessors, the law remains his own, and is something very different from what others had noticed beforehand.

    Back to Table of Contents

    16. The Fundamental Principle.

    Back to Table of Contents

    a

    The articulation of consonants is conditioned by two counteracting factors: The current of air issuing from the lungs, and the tension of the vocal cords and the muscles of the mouth (tongue, lips, velum, cheeks) intercepting the flow. In the case of stops, the breath is completely checked in the mouth-- by the lips in the case of labials (p b), by the tongue in the case of dentals and 'gutturals' (t d; k g). In the case of voiced sounds it is checked in the glottis, by the close approach of the vibrating vocal cords. In some languages, e.g. French, the voiceless stops, p t k, are frequently pronounced with simultaneous closing of the glottis (glottal stop), so that the audible articulation is due to the release of the muscular tension of tongue or lips. We cannot know whether the articulation of IE voiceless stops was originally the same in all parts of the Indo-European territory. It is quite possible that some of the dialects had the glottal stop articulation, but we cannot postulate this for general Indo-European, in spite of Meillet, Caractères généraux des langues germaniques 36 f. For Germanic, we must assume open-glottis articulation, and it matters little whether it represented there an original type or a later development.

    Back to Table of Contents

    b

    In the case of spirants, the oral occlusion is not complete, so that the flow of air is not stopped, but only impeded, to the extent of causing an audible friction between the lips (or lower lip and upper teeth) or between the tongue and the teeth, or the tongue and the roof of the mouth. Again, before passing through the mouth the breath may have been partly intercepted by the vibrating vocal cords. Thus, there result either the voiceless spirants f þ ɧ or the voiced spirants ƀ ð ȝ.

    Back to Table of Contents

    c

    Fortes and Lenes.

    Both stops and spirants vary as to intensity of expiration and muscle tension. Generally speaking, voiceless stops and spirants tend to be fortes, i.e., they are articulated with greater force, and voiced sounds are generally lenes, i.e., the speech organs are relatively relaxed. But a voiced sound can be (relatively) fortis, e.g., bb gg in Ger. Ebbe, Flagge, while a voiceless consonant is sometimes lenis, as South German b d g. In general, it is not necessary to indicate the difference, but where this is required, the following symbols may be used:

    χ ȝ are really the symbols for velar sounds, but generally may also be used for the corresponding palatals, since, in the Germanic languages, the two groups practically amount to one phoneme. But where necessary, palatal articulation may be indicated by ç j: Ger. nicht, liegen = [niçt, li:jən] (but Nacht = [naχt], lagen = [la:ȝən].

    Back to Table of Contents

    d

    All Indo-European stops and spirants pass through a consistent system of changes during the early history of the Germanic languages. In concrete terms, defining merely the actual result, its general course may be described as follows:

    In the case of stops, the breath is released.

    In the case of spirants, the breath is checked.

    Both release and check follow the direction of the breath passing from the lungs, i.e., either process takes place first in the glottis, then in the mouth, as far as this is possible. An occlusion is opened in the case of stops, and a passage is closed in the case of spirants, but of course an open glottis cannot become opened, and a closed mouth cannot become closed. For instance: t is a voiceless stop. The glottis is open. Consequently the breath is released between tongue and teeth, and þ results. This being a voiceless spirant, the open glottis is closed, so that þ changes to ð. This voiced spirant requires an occlusion in the mouth, thus changing to d, a voiced stop, which then, by release in the glottis, becomes t. Doubtless we must assume certain intermediate steps as phonetic probabilities. Thus, t was probably in Pre-Germanic, as elsewhere in Indo-European, a pure stop, and at first the air pressure increased so that the aspirate t' resulted as a transition between t and þ; possibly there existed for a certain time, and in certain positions, a further transitional step, the 'affricate' . þ was probably originally a fortis, being articulated with that force of expiration and counter acting muscle tension that had led to its formation. But this double maximum of intensity was transitory; the sound became lenis. Between d and t we are bound to assume as a phonetic interpolation the voiceless lenis . Therefore, the complete series of these changes is the following:

    t > t' > þ > ð > d > ḍ > t

    Back to Table of Contents

    e

    Grimmm's description of the process is generally represented in this form

    M = media (b d g)

    T = tenuis (p t k)

    A = aspirata; in this term Grimm includes both the Gmc. spirants f þ ɧ and the Gk. 'aspirates', as representatives of what are now termed IE bh dh gh. This nomenclature, while phonetically inaccurate, is natural enough in a chapter that is inscribed 'Lehre von den Buchstaben', and in point of fact there is a certain practical simplicity in the application of this diagram.

    Back to Table of Contents

    f

    The following diagram may, in a sense, be called an adaptation of Grimm's diagram to phonetic facts, but it is correct only if the IE 'sonant aspirates' are assumed to have been voiceless spirants in lenis articulation (the arrows denote the direction of the changes; the figures correspond to the arrangement of the following table):

    Anticipating sections 18-21 and 24-27, we obtain on the basis of this diagram, the following chronology of the consonant shift, examples being taken from the dentals, since their shift is carried farthest:

    I. Pre-Germanic: θ (= dh) > ð > Gk. θίρα: Gmc. *ðurā- (E. door)

    II. Germanic: B.C., t > t' > þ— L. tres: Go þreis, E. three

    III. Germanic: A.D., 'Verner's Law'— þ > ð— Gk. ματήρ: Go. faðar (spelled fadar)

    IV. Germanic: A.D., d > t— L. edo: Go. itan, E. eat

    V. Intermediate: ð > d— Gmc. *ðurā- > Go. daur; Go. faðar: fæder

    VI. High German: t > z— Go. itan: OHG. ezzan

    VII. High German: d > (> t)—Go. daur: OHG. dor, tor

    VIII. 'German: þ > ð— Go. þreis: OHG. dhrī Isidor)

    IX. 'German': ð > d— OHG. dhrī > drī.

    Back to Table of Contents

    17.

    Back to Table of Contents

    a

    The Cause and Time of the Consonant Shift are moot questions but it is probable that they bear a relation to each other. It is hardly by mere accident that this strikingly comprehensive and homogeneous group of phonetic changes is contemporaneous with what may justly be called the most momentous national movement in history: the Germanic Migrations ('Völkerwanderung'), which began at least as early as the second century B.C., perhaps much earlier, and ended during the sixth century of our Era, so that its whole duration was not much less than a millennium.

    Back to Table of Contents

    b

    The estimates of the Time of the consonant shift show a wide difference of opinion. Kluge represents one extreme (Urgermanisch 522) : the second pre-Christian millennium, and Much the other (Btr. 17. 63) : the third century B.C. Between these two estimates stands that of Bethge, Altg. Diai. 176: between 1000 and 400 B.C. This view is approved by H. Meyer, ZfdA. 45. 126, and Kossinna, IF 4. 49, who fix the date at about 400. The only concrete arguments consist in loan words and proper names, but the dating of the former is generally uncertain, and the possibility of sound substitution exists in both types of words. E.g., names like Cimbri, Teutones, with L. c t for Germanic spirants, do not indicate that the consonant shift had not yet taken place at the time of the Cimbrian migration; rather, these consonants are either Roman or Celtic substitutions. Cf. Hirt, Indogermanen, 2. 616; Kluge, i. c. 51 f.; Hirt, HU 1. 101 ff. Apparently the best attempt at a solution is a pragmatic hypothesis that is in keeping with phonetic as well as historical facts, but any such construction must necessarily be uncertain. In that sense, an analysis of the chronology above leads to the following conclusions:

    Back to Table of Contents

    c

    Step I: IE bh dh gh (= φ θ χ) became b d g in all IE languages except Italic, Hellenic,' and Indie. In Germanic, where the transition is incomplete, an intermediate stage ƀ ð ȝ is certain, in the other languages that have b d g, such a phonetic interpolation is very probable. This transition is such a frequent and obvious physiological process that it does not entitle us to consider the b- d- g-languages as a prehistoric unit. The change occurred independently, but it is not a part of the Germanic consonant shift proper, although it must be included in its consideration. It must be ascribed to 'Pre-Germanic' times, between the separation of the Germanic from the Italic group and the actual consonant shift, and therefore, at a random estimate, may be said to have taken place around 1000 B.C.

    Steps II and IV : The changes p t k > f þ ɧ and b d g > p t k represent a surplus of expiration over the counteracting muscle tension, either in the mouth, or in the glottis. They seem to be connected, both as an effect and as a contributing cause, with the Pre-Germanic accent change: the IE accent was musical (pitch accent), and the Gmc. accent was dynamic (stress accent) (43). This view was first expressed by Hirt, Indogermanen, 2. 616, and repeated by R. C. Boer, Neophilologus 1. 103, and Oerg. Hb. 136 f. The change of p t k to f þ ɧ cannot have been a sudden transition, but implied a gradual strengthening of expiration that extended over generations, perhaps over centuries. If we try to indicate, not its beginning, but its completion, Much's date, the third century B.C., would seem acceptable. The unvoicing of the voiced stops must have been still later; here at least we have fairly definite evidence from loan words: words like Go. Krēks (L. Graecus), Go. paida (Gk. βαίτη), Go. kanaps (L. cannabis), whatever the way of borrowing may have been, indicate that the transition was not completed before the end of the pre-Christian Era. Cf. Hirt, HU 1. 102.

    Step III : Verner's Law (20) must have been almost contemporaneous with Step IV and may be ascribed to the first generations of the Christian Era-— an entirely tentative estimate.

    Steps V to IX occurred during the individual history of the several Germanic languages, although at least Step V started in Germanic times. In these changes the following facts of relative chronology become cletfrly evident:

    (1)The 'Intermediate Shift', i.e., the change from Prim. Gmc. ƀ ð ȝ to b d g, took place sooner in initial than in medial or final position. This change implies an increase in muscular tension, which normally is greater initially than medially. Some Romance languages exhibit a corresponding change in the opposite direction, from stop to spirant. Go. [siƀun] is the earlier form, and OHG. sibun the later one, but L.debēre became [deƀer] in Spanish, devoir in French. Prim. Gmc. ƀerana-yielded Go. bairan, and b in L. bonus remained b in Standard French, Spanish, Italian. In both groups, then, the stop is more apt to prevail in initial position, the spirant medially, although the Romance languages change the medial stop to a spirant, Germanic the medial spirant to a stop.

    The recognition of the Germanic voiced spirants is due to Braune, Btr. 1. 1 ff. (Zur Kenntnis des Fränkischen und zur hochdeutschen Lautverschiebung). The view of Sweet, Meillet, Huchon, that Germanic had voiced stops, which became spirants medially and finally, is clearly wrong.

    (2) In the 'High German Shift', p t k became spirants in medial or final position, but affricates (pf ts kh) in initial or post-consonantal position: E. open, eat, make =offen, essen, machen, but pound, ten, corn = Pfund, zehn [tse:n], Early UG khorn (Standard German Korn). The surplus of expiration over tension overcame the occlusion more completely after vowels, where the tension was weaker.

    (3) In all phases of the Intermediate and High German Shifts dentals change first and most completely, labials later and less, and velars last and least. This is particularly marked in the High German Shift of b d g to p t k. Everywhere in Upper German, d became t, and this change extended also over the larger part of Middle German; the changes b > p and g > k occurred only in Upper German, but the former was much more far-reaching than the latter. Phonetically, this seems to mean that the ratio of change is proportional to the relative agility of the articulating organs: the tip of the tongue is the most agile, the back of the tongue and the velum are the most inert of the three articulating organs. It is not quite impossible that the relative frequency of sounds may also have played a part, at least in the case of the dentals, which are the most frequent among consonants, but on the other hand, velars are more frequent than labials, which speaks against that assumption.

    Back to Table of Contents

    d

    Residuary Forms ('Restwörter'). In general, phonetic laws are carried through 'without exceptions' (cf. 9), but phonetic laws are limited in their duration. For instance, in Greek intervocalic s disappeared: γενεσος > γένους. Later, intervocalic t became s under certain conditions, and this new s remained: *λυτις > λύσις. In Germanic as well as in other languages it happened occasionally that a phonetic law came to a standstill before it had covered the entire vocabulary of the language. Especially connectives and other words of relatively neutral semantic contents appear as such linguistic residue. Thus in Gothic, the shift from d to t did not affect the prepositidu 'to' and the prefix dis- 'asunder', while *dwōi, *dek^ṃ(t) had become twai, taíhun. In Middle Franconian, postvocalic t became regularly zz, as in *watar > wazzar; but in pronominal forms and endings -t remained: that, et, wat, allet.

    The very existence of such residuary forms ('Restwörter' ; cf. Prokosch, Mod. Phil. 26. 459 f.) seems to confirm the theory that the consonant shift had its origin in the emphatic articulation of stressed words and syllables, and this is in keeping with the order described above, in which the consonants changed according to their place of articulation and their position in the word. The 'Germanic Consonant Shift' took place in pre-historic times and was virtually completed at the time to which our earliest records belong. But the 'Intermediate' and the 'High German' shifts belong to a period of expansion and migration, the main events of which are sufficiently well known to establish a parallelism with the linguistic changes of that period, and it is fairly safe to assume that the pre-historic developments would show a similar parallelism if the details could be ascertained.

    Back to Table of Contents

    e

    The Cause of the consonant shift is even more problematic than its time. Jacob Grimm gave a purely psychological explanation which, in spite of the Romantic exuberance of its wording, may fundamentally contain a good deal of truth. He sees in the consonant shift (the Ger manic as well as the High German) an expression of the impetuous character of the Germanic tribes during their early history. H. Meyer(-Benfey) advanced the theory that at least a large part of it was due to an increase in the force of expiration, brought about by life in mountainous districts: the Scandinavian highlands for the Germanic, the Alps for the High-German Shift. His theory has found considerable approval, expressed, for instance, by Osthoff in his lectures, and by Collitz, Language 2. 181. Lately, the 'ethnological theory' seems to be the leading view. According to this, the Germanic as well as the High German Shifts are due to language mixture between Pre-Germanic or Germanic, and Celtic, Finnic, Rhaeto-Etruscan, or the speech of an autochthonous population of North-Central Europe.

    In the author's opinion, H. Meyer's view is not without a measure of intrinsic probability. But his factual arguments, showing similar phonetic processes among other mountain tribes, are scattered and insufficient. Moreover, his two chief postulates are highly problematic : If the original Germanic home was on the Scandinavian peninsula, it was not upon the high fjällar, but in comparatively level southern Sweden. This speaks against Meyer's explanation of the Germanic shift. As to the High German shift, Braune's assumption that it started in the Alps (see below) is a mere hypothesis without historical foundation. Should it happen to be correct, it would be difficult to understand why the Romance languages of the Western Alps, or the languages of the Caucasus or the Himalaya do not show any similar phenomena. Meyer's physiological explanation of certain parts of the consonant shift as the result of increased expiration is a valuable supple ment to Grimm and Raumer, but this increase cannot have been due to life in the mountains.

    The varieties of the ethnological theory are too numerous to discuss in detail. In spite of some brilliant observations (especially by Kauffmann and Naumann) all of them are weak in principle. On the one hand, voiceless stops are supposed to change to spirants under the influence of absorbed languages (none of which, as far as is known, possessed such sounds to a similar extent), and on the other, for the same reason, voiced stops became voiceless. Moreover, the very multitude of guesses is self-defeating. The phonetic basis of Finnic, e.g., differs radically from that of Celtic; about Rhaeto-Etruscan (if such a language group existed) we know very little, and about the mythical autochthonous language of North-Central Europe nothing at all. Finally, the thorough consistency of this wide complex of phonetic changes is a decisive argument against foreign influence as a determining cause. Any one of the individual steps might, by some stretch of imagination, be ascribed to such— say, the spirantization of occlusives to Celtic,7 the unvoicing of b d g to Finnic or Rhaetian; but these influences would necessarily have acted independently of each other, and it is quite incredible that the phonetic laws resulting from these separate influences would accidentally have fallen into such a homogeneous pattern as is described in the 'Fundamental Principle', 16.

    Back to Table of Contents

    f

    The following attempt at a solution of the problem is far from adeuate, since our knowledge of the historical foundations is incomplete; but it tries to avoid the worst fallacies of the geographical and ethnological theories; the historical details (based essentially on Lamprecht's Deutsche Geschichte) are given by Prokosch, Die deutsche Lautverschiebung und die Völkerwanderung, JEGPh 16. 1 ff.

    (1) The Consonant Shift is one continuous process, the duration of which is approximately parallel to that of the Germanic Migration in the wider sense: it began several centuries B.C. and ended about 500 A.D. The Second or High German Shift continues or duplicates in a consistent way the phonetic processes of the Germanic Shift.

    (2) The basic trend of the consonant shift developed in and near the Germanic homeland: Scadinavia, i.e. the West Baltic Basin, and Northern Germany, between Elbe and Oder. In accordance with 7, Chronology and Drift, it ceased in each tribe soon after the final settlement in the new home. Accordingly, as the following sections will show, the scope of the shift in each dialect corresponds to the relative period of colonization.

    (3) These two statements are based on facts and are merely descriptive of what actually took place. But what was the cause of this 'Basic Trend'? Here we can offer only a hypothetical construction. Linguistic change is largely due to imitation. Every individual departs at times or always from the average norm of speech. To an extent these personal deviations are the result of character and circumstances. Leading personalities are apt to be imitated in their manners, their dress, their speech: they set fashions.

    The Germanic Migration was the result of overpopulation of comparatively infertile land, aggravated in the northwest by inundations, and possibly in the east by pressure from Slavs and other nations. Emigration brought temporary relief for some districts, but in general the necessity of emigration continued in the homeland through these centuries. Every expedition required years of preparation and organiszation, and such continued storm and stress could not fail to develop leaders whose personalities influenced the 'behavior' of their followers. It can easily be imagined that in personalities of this type will and contents predominated over reflection and form: the influence of a Boiorix or Ariovistus upon speech as a part of behavior differed from that of a Petronius or Marcus Aurelius. Not only the consonant shift, but also the accent shift, the vowel shift, the Germanic verb system, all reflected a predominance of elements of contents over elements of form.

    At any rate, the consonant shift appears to stand in chronological and causal relation to the social, economic, and emotional background of the Völkerwanderung. These chronological facts are certain (cf. 7): East and North Germanic, the oldest branches from the point of view of settlement in new homes, show only steps I-IV and a small part of step V. West Germanic north of the Benrather Linie (6 e) continues step V. High German, particularly Upper German, in the most recent Germanic settlements, completes step V and adds steps VI and VII. The parallelism is too close to be accidental.

    The Germanic Shift

    Back to Table of Contents

    18.

    The IE Spirants, φ θ χ conventional bh dh gh, appear in Germanic as voiced spirants, ƀ ð ȝ: Step I. For their origin and transcription, cf. 10 (4). It is generally asserted that the shift ph th kh > f þ ɧ and that of bh dh gh > ƀ ð ȝ are perfect phonetic parallels. This is by no means the case, in spite of the deceptive appearance of the Schriftbild. In Germanic, voiced and unvoiced sounds move in opposite directions, although the underlying 'drift' is the same. If IE bh dh gh had existed and had developed in accordance with the general trend, they would have become ph th kh— a transition that is generally, although perhaps wrongly, assumed for Hellenic and Italic, but which certainly did not take place in Germanic.

    If we accept φ θ χ as tentative reconstructions, we find complete consistency. In accordance with the 'Fundamental Principle' (16) the breath is checked in the glottis, and Gmc. ƀ ð ȝ result, exactly as later in step III. This stage is generally assumed for Primitive Germanic ('Urgermanisch'); cf. especially Braune, Btr. 1. 1 ff. But even there the transition to stops had begun, at least in gemination and after nasals: theoretical ƀƀ ðð ȝȝ, mƀ nð ŋȝ (24) appear everywhere as bb dd gg, mb nd ng. The reason is obvious: gemination implies increased tension, and nasals are essentially stops, so that assimilation results. Probably also initial b d are to be ascribed to the Germanic period, although possibly some very early Runic inscriptions indicate spirantic pronunciation: ƀarutʀ, Bjorketorp = brȳtr; ðaȝaʀ , Einag = dagr; (Noreen, Aisl. Gramm. 143).

    --

    Examples (cf . 10, 4):

    φ = bh:*bher-, Go. bairan, bera, OE OS OHG. beran

    *bhātēr, Go. brōþar, brōþer, brōþor, OS brōðar, OHG. bruoder

    *nebh-, nifl- 'night', nifol 'dark', OS rubal, OHG nebvi 'fog'

    θ = dh:*dhē-, Go. ga-dēþs, dāþ, dǣd, OS dād, OHG. tāt 'deed'

    *roudho-, Go. rauða-, rauþr, rēad, OS rōd, OHG. rōt

    *medhjo-, Go. miðjis, miþr, mid, OS middi, OHG. mitti

    χ = ĝh:*ĝhḷtó, Go. gulþ, gull, gold, OS OHG. gold, OSi. zlato 'gold'

    *ĝheu(d)-, Go. giutan, giōta, gēotan, OS giotan, OHG. giozan

    ĝheŋgh-, Go. gaggan, ganga, gongan, OS OHG. gangan

    *weĝh-, Go. -wigan, vega, OE OHG. wegan

    χ = gh:*ghosti-, Go. gasts, gestr, giest, OS OHG. gasts

    *ghorto-, Go. gards, garþr, geard, OS gard, OHG gart (garto)

    *steigh-, Go. steigan, stīga, OE OS OHG. stīgan

    χw = ghw: *ghwṇtjā 'killing', gunnr, OHG. gund- 'fight'.

    Note: The pronunciation of b d g in the various Germanic languages is discussed in 24. In the Gothic examples given above, ð is used for medial d in Gothic spelling, since its spirantic character is quite certain. After this, d will be used (fadar, instead of faðar).

    Back to Table of Contents

    19.

    The IE Voiceless Stops, p t k, became Gmc. f þ ɧ, with p' t' k' as intermediate stage: Step II. According to 16 c these spirants were originally fortes, but in historical times they are, relatively, lenes. This is apparent from the following facts: In Gothic they remain voiceless in all positions (broþar, wulfis 'wolf's', taihun 'ten'). In Old Norse and Old English, f and þ in voiced surroundings become voiced, which presupposes lenis articulation; in both languages, the letter f is used for f and v, and þ and ð are used indiscriminately for the voiceless and voiced spirants; cf. 27 b: brōþor, brōþer = brōðor, brōðer are pronounced with ð, regardless of the spelling. The same is true for OS th (brōthar has the sound ð), but the voiced labial is usually spelled ƀ in Old Saxon : neƀo 'nephew'. In Old High German, medial Gmc. f is spelled f or v (nefo, nevo 'nephew') and was probably voiceless, but lenis, sharply distinguished from OHG. f < Gmc. p, which is generally spelled ff: offan 'open' (but ovan 'oven'). As to the development of þ in Old High German, see 27 b. For the Gmc. voiceless velar spirant this book uses the symbol ɧ, a modified h: it had originally the value of ch (probably in both pronunciations, according to the preceding or following vowel), but became h or disappeared in most positions. Roman historians of the older period express it by ch: Chariowaldus, Cherusci, or c: Cimbri. In the historical dialects it is weakened to the glottal spirant in initial position (Go. harjis 'Heer'); medially and finally it tends to disappear: Go. taihun, OHG. zehan, but ON tīo, tīen 'ten'. For details, see 27 d. It had become voiced ȝ in Prim. Gmc. under Verner's Law (20), but in historical times this process was not repeated.

    Examples (cf. 10, 1):

    p: *pətēr, Go. fadar, faþer, fæder, OS fadar, OHG. fater

    *pōd-, Go. fōtus, fōtr, OE OS fōt, OHG. fuoz

    *penkwe, Go. fimf, fimm, OE OS fīf, OHG. fimf

    t:*treies, Go. þreis, þrīr, þrīe, OS thrie, OHG. drī

    *tū, Go. ON þū OS thu, OHG.

    tod, Go. þata, þat, þæt, OS that, OHG. daz

    k^:*k^mtóm, Go. hund, hund(raþ), OE OS hund, OHG. hunt

    *dek^ṃ, Go. taihun, tiō, tien, OS tehan, OHG. zehan

    *deik^-, Go. teihan, tiō, tēon, OS -tīhan, OHG. zīhan

    k:*kən-/kōn-, Go. hana, hane, hana, OS OHG. hano 'rooster', L. canō, ci-cōnia

    *kāro-, Go. hōrs, ON OE OS hōr, OHG. huor 'adulterer, -y', L. cārus

    *leuk-, Go. liuhaþ, leoht, OS OHG. lioht 'light', L. lūc-s

    kw:*kwod, Go. ƕa, huat, hwæt, OS hwat, OHG. hwaz

    *sekw-, Go. saiƕan, siā, sēon, OS OHG. sehan

    *leikw-, Go. leiƕan, liā, -lēon, OS OHG. līhan 'lend', L. līnquō.

    There is no Germanic evidence for IE aspirated voiceless stops.

    Note: After voiceless spirants, i.e., Gmc. f þ ɧ and s, this shift did not take place, apparently because the surplus expiration was absorbed by these sounds. In a sense, we can speak of dissimilation: L. -spiciō, OHG. spehōn 'spy', L. stella, OHG. stern(o) 'star', L. scindō, Go. skaidan 'separate', L. neptis, OHG. niftila, NHG (borrowing from Low German) Nichte 'niece', L. octō, Go. ahtau 'eight'. In some cases, this Gmc. spirant goes back to IE b bh, g gh before a voiceless consonant, e.g. L. regōrictus, Go. raihts 'right'; IE ĝhoŋghti-, Go. gāhts 'walk', IE məghti-, Go. mahts 'might'. — For words like IE keudh-tó-, Go. huzd 'hoard' cf. 28.

    Back to Table of Contents

    20.

    Back to Table of Contents

    a

    Verner's Law

    After Step II of the Consonant Shift, Primitive Germanic, like Primitive Indo-European, had four voiceless spirants, namely, f þ ɧ < IE p t k, and s = IE s. In voiced surroundings, these tend to become voiced, ƀ ð ȝ z. The same trend is frequently found in other languages. Thus IE bh dh are voiceless in Umbr. tefe, rufru, Osc. mefiai, but voiced in L. tibi, rubro, mediae; IE ĝenesā > Italic genezā > L. genera. Gothic has a voiceless spirant in brōþar, wairþan, but the corresponding Norse and West-Germanic words show voicing: ON [brōðer, verða], OE [brōðor, weorðan]. In many instances, however, the voicing dates back to Primitive Germanic times: Go. sibun [siƀun], fadar [faðar], tigjus [tiȝjus], seofon, fæder, -tig. The divergence itself had been recognized by Grimm, who coined the term 'Grammatischer Wechsel' (see below) for its most characteristic type. W. Braune, Btr. 1. 513-27 (1873), systematized this group of forms ('Ueber den grammatischen Wechsel in der deutschen Verbalflexion'), without, however, offering any explanation of the apparent irregularity. The reason was approximately recognized by E. Sievers in 1874, who stated it in a letter to Braune (published by H. Osthoff, Die neueste Sprachforschung und die Erklarung des indogermanischen Ablauts 13, Heidelberg 1886), but did not publish it. In 1877, the Danish linguist Karl Verner found the cause of the divergence independently and published it in KZ 23. 97 ff., under the title 'Eine Ausnahme der germanischen Lautverschiebung'. He showed that the Germanic strong verb has voiceless spirants in medial position in those forms where in Sanskrit the accent is on the root, but voiced spirants, where Sanskrit has the accent on the ending; Sk. vártati 'I turn', vavárta 'I have turned', vavṛtimá 'we have turned', vartāná- 'turned' (past part.) —Gmc. werþið, warþ: wurðum, wurðan-.

    Starting with this observation, Verner formulated the following law: The Germanic voiceless spirants remained voiceless, if the preceding syllable had the IE accent, but became voiced in voiced surroundings. if the preceding syllable had been unstressed in IE times. (Sievers: 'Im Nachlaut der indogermanisch unbetonten Silbe'). Paul (Btr. 6. 538) gave the following wording: 'Die nach Vollzug der germanischen Lautverschiebung vorhandenen vier harten Reibelaute h þ f s sind ausser in den Verbindungen ht hs ft fs sk st sp erweicht, wenn der nachst vorhergehende Sonant nicht nach der idg. Betonung den Hauptton trug.'1

    -—This is Step III of the Consonant Shift.

    We find the same sound change in Modern Englist in such forms as exámine, extért (with gz) as compared éxit,éxercise (with ks). In spite of this well established parallel, the phonetic explanation is difficult. Probably we have to assume the following: In medial and final position, the Germanic spirants were relatively weaker than initially; their articulation may have approached that of IE φ θ χ (bh dh gh), but, as in E. exit, it was relatively stronger at the end of a stressed syllable than after an unstressed syllable, as in E. exert. In the latter position, the lenis spirant was readily affected by the vocal vibrations of the surrounding voiced sounds, and thereby became voiced. In a later period, this voicing of spirants in voiced surroundings became general, regardless of accent. In spite of the title of Verner's article, Verner's Law is not an exception to the Consonant Shift, but an acceleration of it. (Collitz, Language 2. 177: 'The instances covered by Verner's Law constitute an exception to Grimm's Law only in the sense of an accelerated action of the latter, caused by the IE accent.') The change of voiceless to voiced spirants follows the general trend of the Shift in accordance with the Fundamental Principle given in 16 d. Spirants being open consonants, an occlusion must take place; the glottis being open, this occlusion must be formed in the glottis. After an originally unstressed syllable, the lenis development, and accordingly the voicing, took place sooner than in the relatively strong position in the transition from a stressed syllable. But gradually this voicing becomes general. Thus, Verner's Law is a typical instance of the very frequent gradual spread of a phonetic law. It starts under the most favorable conditions, but spreads until it has covered the whole field. In the second Shift and in Early English (partly late Old English, partly early Middle English) we find an apparently different, but really analogous process in the treatment of th. It became voiced (in voiced surroundings) in medial position without exception (clothes, bathing), but initially only in words which are relatively unstressed in a sentence, or which have little semantic function: the, that, this, then, there, thou, thee, thine. Here the whole word is 'lenis', and therefore the weak spirant is voiced; in the case of Verner's Law, the spirant was lenis because it continued the degree of strength of the preceding syllable.

    -—Scandinavian shows a similar development, beginning in the thirteenth century: Initial þ, appears regularly as t (þing > ting), but as d in the same type of words that have ð in English (de, dem, den, det, der, du, dig, din, etc.).

    Note: R. C. Boer, Neophil. 1. 110, and Oerg. Hb. 123 f., ascribes Verner's Law to double accentuation in Primitive Germanic. He assumes that the syllable that had been stressed in IE times, but became unetressed through the Germanic accent change (48), retained higher pitch. For instance, IE pətēr > Gmc. faþǣr > faðǣr would, in Germanic, have a dynamic accent of the first syllable, but higher pitch on the second syllable. Inherently, there is nothing impossible in this; we do find similar conditions in modern Swedish and Serbian. But the explanation falls short of explaining Verner's Law in the case of final consonants, as well as the analogous processes of later periods in English, Scandinavian, and Old High German (see 27).

    A similar hypothesis had been advanced by R. Gauthiot, MSL 11. 193-7; Prokosch, JEGPh. 11. 1 ff., tries to show that Verner's Law can be explained on the basis of dynamic accent. Cf . also Kip, MLN 20. 16 f.

    ---

    Back to Table of Contents

    b. Chronology

    Unless Boer's theory be accepted, Verner's Law must have preceded the Germanic accent shift (43). This would make the first or second century A.D. the most probable time. At that period, the separation of the Goths from the western and northern Germanic tribes was under way. Now, in Gothic, Verner's Law is much more rare than in the other. Germanic languages (see below). It is commonly assumed that it had existed there to the same extent, but was 'leveled out'. But it is more probable that Hirt, HU 1.148 (cf. also 155) is right when he says 'Ich halte es fur unmöglich, alles dies auf Ausgleichung zurückzuführen.' He assumes that Gothic had carried out the accent shift sooner than the other Germanic languages, so that only a comparatively small group of rather isolated words were still subject to the law. That is quite possible, and I have held a similar view for many years. The accent shift, like other phonetic laws, was a gradual process. There must have been a time when individual speakers would, in the same word, use sometimes the Germanic, sometimes the Indo-European accent. This was favorable to the development of a semiconscious differentiation between the two accent types, resulting in Verner's Law.But at approximately that time the phonetic drift in Gothic was retarded through its separation from the main stock (cf. 7 and 9). In general, the spirant remained voiceless in words where the preceding syllable was stressed in some forms, unstressed in others. But it was voiced where the preceding syllable was unstressed throughout. Thus, Gothic has wairþan, warþ, waurþum, waurþans, although the root was originally unstressed in the last two forms. But Verner's Law did take effect in words like sibun < *septḿ, fadar < *pətēr, where the suffix accent had become fixed in IE or very early Gmc. times. Of course, even so we may speak of a sort of leveling, but only in the sense that the analogy of other forms of the same word prevented the change of waurþun to *waurdun. —The instances of Verner's Law in Gothic are listed by F. A. Wood, Verner's Law in Gothic, 1895.

    Back to Table of Contents

    c. Instances

    Due to later consonant changes and to peculiarities of spelling, Verner's Law is not equally traceable in the several Germanic dialects. The table below shows that, in general, only Gothic, Old Saxon, and Old High German are apt to give evidence of V.L. in all four places of articulation. ON distinguished neither the labial nor the dental spirants; OE did not distinguish the labials. However, the table does not take into account some special cases, such as the change of to nn (finna: Go. finþan) and > ll, but > ld (ellre: aldenn) ; Heusler, Aisl. Elb. §159. In ON and f denotes both the voiceless and the voiced spirant, according to position, and the letters þ and ð are used interchangeably, also denoting either the voiceless or the voiced spirant, according to position (27 b, c). The table applies to medial or final position only.

    Gmc.
    f
    =
    Go.
    f
    ON
    f
    OE
    f
    OS
    f
    OHG
    f
    " ƀ
    " b
    " f " f " ƀ
    " b
    " þ
    "
    þ " þ ð
    " þ ð
    " th
    " d
    " ð
    " d " þ ð
    " þ ð
    " d
    " t
    " ɧ " h
    " - " -, h
    " h
    " h
    " ȝ
    " g "
    g "
    g
    " g " g
    " s
    " s "
    s " s " s " s
    " z " z " r " r " r " r

    In the following instances leveling, including the assumed retention of voiceless spirants in Gothic, is indicated by brackets. For a more complete list, see Streitberg, UG 127 ff., and Noreen, UL 124 ff. Cf. also 63.

    Non-Gmc. Go. ON OE OS OHG
    f > ƀ Sk. tarpáyati þaurban þurfa þurfan thurban durfan
    Gk. επτά sibun siau1 seofon sibun sibun
    L. caput haubiþ hǫfoþ hēafod hōbid houbit
    þ > ð Gk. εκατόν hund hund hund hund hunt
    L. altus alds (noun) aldenn eald ald alt
    Gk. πατήρ fadar faþer fæder fadar fater
    ɧ > ȝ Gk. δεκάς tigus tiger -tig -tig -zug
    L. dūco [taúhans] togenn togen gitogan gizogan
    L. cunctārī [hāhan] hanga hangian hangon hangēn
    s > z L. auris [ausō] eyra ēare ōra ōra
    Osc. mais maiza meire māra mēro mēro
    L. -ōrum þizē þeira þāra thero dero

    Back to Table of Contents

    d.

    Grammatical Change is primarily the effect of the application or non-application of Verner's Law in different inflectional forms of the same word, due to the movable accent of Indo-European (43), but in a wider sense the term is often also applied to the alternation of Gmc. voiceless and voiced spirants in different words of like formation, or in the same word in different Gmc. languages. To the first class belongs, e.g., OE pres. cēosan 'choose', past part. coren; to the second class, on the one hand, Go. fadar < *pətēr against brōþar < *bhrātēr, and on the other hand, Go. dauþa- against dēad 'dead'.

    The following types of Grammatical Change are especially important:

    (1)For the principal parts of the strong verb the standard view assumes that in all seven classes the first two forms (pres. and singular of the pret.) had in IE root accent, the other two (plural of the pret. and past part.) suffix accent. As shown in 63, this is probably true only for the first three classes, but through analogical transfer Grammatical Change does occur, more or less sporadically, also in the fifth, sixth, and seventh classes. Gothic retains (or restores?) the voiceless spirant in the regular strong verb, but Grammatical Change is found in some forms of the preterit-presents.

    Instances (analogical forms are given in brackets):

    I. Go. leiþan laiþ [liþum] 'go'
    ON līþa leiþ liþom
    OE līþan lāþ lidon
    OS līthan lēth lidun
    OHG līdan leid litum
    Go. sneiþan snaiþ [sniþum] 'cut'
    ON snīþa sneiþ sniþom
    OE snīþan snāþ snidon
    OS snīthan snēth snidun
    OHG snīdan sneid snitum

    II. Go. kiusan kaus [kusum] 'choose'
    ON kiōsa kaus kørom
    OE cēosan cēas curon
    OS kiosan kōs kurun
    OHG kiosan kōs kurum
    Go. tiuhan tauh [tauhum] 'pull'
    ON tiōa - -
    OE tēon tēah tugon
    OS tiohan tōh tugun
    OHG ziohan zōh zugum

    III. Go. wairþan warþ [waruþum] [waurþans] 'become'
    ON verþa varþ urþom orþenn
    OE weorþan wearþ wurdon worden
    OS werthan warth wurdun wordan
    OHG werdan ward wurtum wortan
    Go. finþan fanþ [funþum] [funþans] 'find'
    ON finna fann fundom fundenn
    OE [findan] [fand] fundon fundenn
    OS fīthan [fand] fundun fundan
    OHG findan fand funtum funtan

    V. Go. wisan was wēsun - 'be'
    ON vesa vas [vǫrom] veret
    OE wesan was [wǣron] -
    OS wesan was [wārun] -
    OHG wesan was [wārum] -
    Go. qiþan qaþ qēþum [qiþans] 'speak'
    ON kueþa kuaþ kuǫþom kueþenn
    OE cweþan cwæþ [cwǣdon] cweden
    OS quethan quath [quādun] giquedan
    OHG quedan quad [quātum] giquetan

    VI. Go. [slahan] slōh slōhum [slahans] 'strike'
    ON [slā] slō [slōgom] slegenn
    OE [slēan] slōh [slōgon] slagen
    OS [slahan] [slōg] [slōgun] gislagen
    OHG [slahan] sluoh [sluogum] gislagan
    Go. [hafjan] hōf hōfum [hafans] 'seize'
    ON hefia hōf hōfom hafenn
    OE hebban hōf hōfon hæfen
    OS hebbian hōf [hōƀun] gihaƀan
    OHG [heffen] [huob] [huobum] gihaban

    VII. Go [fāhan] faifāh faifāhum [fāhans] 'catch'
    ON [fā] [fekk] [fengom] fengenn
    OE [fōn] [fēng] [fēngom] fangen
    OS [fāhan] [feng] [fengum] gifangan
    OHG [fāhan] [fieng] [fiengum] gifangan

    Preterit Presents:

    I. Go. aih aigum infin. aigan 'own'
    ON ā eigom " eiga
    OE āh āgon " āgan
    OS *ēh ēgun " ēgan
    OHG *ēh eigun noun eigan

    III. Go. þarf þaurbum infin. þaurban 'need'
    ON þarf þurfom " þurfa
    OE þearf þurfon " þurfan
    OS tharf thurƀun " thurƀan
    OHG darf [durfum] " [durfan]

    (2)There existed certain present types with IE suffix accent (63 a), notably the aorist presents, the verbs with n-infix, and the causatives. In spite of a great deal of leveling, these still give in some cases evidence of Grammatical Change:

    Aorist Presents:

    I. IE v)et(n)k-/wi(n)k* (L. rtncd): Go. weihan 'fight'—ON vega, v& < *waih, vfgom, vegenn; OE vflgan; OHG vbar-wehan 'conquer'; OE OS uHgand, OHG uHgant 'fighter' II. IE bh£uk-/bhuh. (Lith. buklits 'sly') and bhhig-/bhvg'- (L. fugit): Go. biugan 'bend', ON bogenn (past part.), OE bUgan, OS 'bOgan, OHG biogan, but OHG buhU 'hill' VI. IE kbp-JJap- (L. capid): Go. hafjan, OE hebban, etc. (see above).

    Verbs with n-Infix: IE std-/st& (L. stare—status) : Go. standan, stop, stdpum, standans; ON standa, stdp, stdPom, stapenn; OE standan, [stdd], [stddon], standen; OS standan, [stdd], [stddun], astandan; OHG stantan, [stuont], [stuon-tum], gistantan. IE trenkr.: Go. preihan (transferred to the first class) 'throng', ON pryngua, OE pringan, OS thringan, OHG dringan (Gothic general izes h, the other dialects 5 through all forms).

    Causatives: IE w6rt-e-ti 'he turns', intr., wort-eje-ti 'he makes turn, he turns', trans.; cf. Sk. vdrtati —vartdyati: Go. frawairpan 'spoil', intr., frawardjan 'spoil', trans. Go. leipan, OE Upon, etc. (see above) 'go' —Go. *laidjan 'lead', OE Ixdan, OS ledian, OHG leiten. Go. ganisan, OE genesan, OS OHG ginesan 'recover' —Go. nasjan, OE nerigan, OS nerian, OHG nerien 'save'. Go. lisan 'know' —Go. laisjan, OE Ueran, OS lerian, OHG ISran 'make know, teach'.

    (3) Often the same dialect offers different word formations with original accent variation and, therefore, Grammatical Change:

    OHG. durfan 'need' -—darbēn (Go. gaþarban) 'abstain'

    OHG. hof 'court'—- hūbesch 'courtly, pretty'

    OHG. fāhan 'catch'— fuogen 'join'

    dēaþ, OS dōth, OHG. tōd 'death'— dēad, OS dōd, OHG. tōt 'dead'.

    (4)In other cases, we find differences of the same type between dialects; in such cases, generally, but by no means always, Gothic shows the voiceless spirant: Go. ufar 'over' indicates the accent of Sk. úpa, OHG. ubir that of Sk. upári, Gk. 'υπέρ; Go. alþeis 'old' shows root accent, ON >i>aldenn, eald, OHG. alt, suffix accent (*altós); the stem of the adjective 'dead' is dauþa- in Gothic, but *dauða- in West Germanic: dead, OS dōd, OHG. tōt (dauþr could, theoretically, go with either of the two forms, but doubtless has to be classed with the WGmc. forms). Likewise Go. ganauha 'sufficiency', ganōhs 'enough', but OS ginōg, OHG. ginuog; also Go. fraþi 'sense', gabaurþi- 'birth', gaqumþi- 'meeting', hūhrus 'hunger', and many others have cognates with voiced spirants in West Germanic and Norse. In such cases, the assumption of leveling is not sufficient; we must assume, with Hirt, l. c., that in these words Gothic had root accent sooner than the other Germanic languages. -—Variations between the other Germanic lan guages are less frequent: OHG. haso, but hara, here 'hare'.

    Note: According to Bugge, Btr. 12. 309 ff., 13. 167 ff. and 311 ff., Verner's Law, under certain conditions, also affected initial spirants. Probably, this can be accepted only for the second parts of compounds, such as OHG. mezzirahs (but also mezzisahs) 'eating-knife', Messer < *mati-sahs, OS mezas < *metsahs, meteseax. However, the equation of Gmc. ga-, gi- with L. co(n) seems to find justification in the assumption of Verner's Law initially, in the same sense in which we may apply the term to E. the, that, Norse de, det, etc. (see above). Unstressed Gmc. ɧa- had lenis pronunciation at an earlier period than accented syllables with initial ɧ, and thereby became subject to the operation of Verner's Law.

    Back to Table of Contents

    21.

    The IE Voiced Stops, b d g, became voiceless: p t k— Step IV. The intermediate stage was doubtless the voiceless lenis, ḅ ḍ .g. Aside from the obvious phonetic probability, this is shown by the parallelism of the second shift (26). In general p t k are aspirated fortes in the Germanic languages, but exceptions exist in Dutch and High German.

    It is not known why b was so extremely rare in Indo-European, but it can hardly have been entirely accidental. Balto-Germanic cognates with IE b are fairly numerous, and Slavo-Germanic cognates almost equally so. But there are extremely few reliable etymologies of this kind that can be called 'Indo-European'. Perhaps a consideration like this may, in the course of time, lead to an understanding of the problem: 'Indo-European' is just as little a fixed linguistic system as is 'Germanic', or 'Slavic', or 'Indic'. It had been in flux for an indefinite period before the time of our reconstruction, which is necessarily fixed in an arbitrary way. There are indications that it was going through a pre-historic process similar to the Germanic Consonant Shift. According to 24, the stop b < ƀ was rare at a given period in early Germanic, on account of the chronological difference in the development of the three places of articulation. Perhaps something similar had taken place in early Indo-European. The voiced dental spirants, precursors of d, had perhaps already completed their transition; the 'gutturals' may still have been in the spirantic stage, so that IE χ(gh) would represent an earlier chronological stage than θ(dh), just as k does not correspond to t, which is a later development (k — Gmc. k, IE g; t — Gmc. d, IE θ). The IE labials may have been approaching the occluding stage at the time of the dialect separation. All of this is admittedly vague and open to some obvious objections, but we can hardly get any farther until new methods are devised.

    Instances: (Cf. 19, 3)

    b:IE skəb-/skāb-, L. scabō, Go. skapjan, skape, scieppan, OS -scapan, OHG. scepfen 'shape' (but also the root doublet IE skəbh-, Go. skaban, OHG. scaban 'scrape'

    IE terb-/trb-, L. turba 'troup', Go. þaurp 'field', þorp, þorp, OS thorp, OHG. dorf 'village'

    IE dheub-/dheup-, lith. dibùs, Go. diups, diūpr, dēop, OS diop, OHG. tiof 'deep' (but also dūfa, dȳfan 'dive')

    d: IE deˆkṃ, L. decem, Go. taihun, tiō, tīen, OS tehan, OHG. zehan 'ten'

    IE dwōi (dwōu), L. duo, Go. twai, tveir, twā, OS twā, OHG. zwei 'two'

    IE dom-, L. domus, Go. timrjan, timbra, timbrian, OS timbrōn, OHG. zimbarōn 'build'

    ĝ: IE ĝeus-, L. gustāre, Go. kiusan, kiōsa, cēosan, OS kiosan, OHG. kiosan 'choose'

    IE ĝen-, L. genus, Go. kuni, kyn, cynn, OS kunni, OHG. kunni 'kin'

    IE ĝneu-, L. genu, Go. kniu, kni, cnēo, OHG. kniu 'knee'

    g: IE aug-, L. augeō, Go. aukan, auka, ēacian, OS ōkian, OHG. ouhhon 'increase'

    IE gel-, L. gelidus, Go. kalds, kalþr, ceald, OS kald, OHG. kalt 'cold'

    IE jugo-, L. iugum, Go. juk, ok, geoc, OS juk, OHG. joch 'yoke'

    gw: IE gwem-, L. veniō, Go. qiman, kona, cuman, OS kuman, OHG. koman 'come'

    IE gwīno-, Gk. γυνή, Go. qinō, kona, cwene, OS OHG. quena 'woman'

    IE gwīwo-, L. vīvus, Go. qius, kuikr, cwic(u), OS quick, OHG. queck 'alive'.

    Back to Table of Contents

    22.

    The Germanic Consonant Lengthening.

    Indo-European possessed no original 'double' or 'long' consonants (geminates), but doubling resulted frequently from composition or derivation: IE wid-to, wit-to- 'known' = OHG. giwisso. The Germanic development of such groups is treated in 29, but one type is so closely connected with Steps III and IV of the Consonant Shift that it is anticipated here. This is the treatment of Germanic stops and spirants followed by n. Such groups occurred chiefly in certain derivative verbs, such as Gk δάκνω, Sk. krīṇāmi, and in the 'weak cases' of the n-declension, such as Gk. 'αρνός, gen. sg. of 'αρήν 'ram'. There are so many irregularities and so few non-Germanic equations in these forms that as yet complete agreement has not been reached, but the following must be considered the standard view: IE p t k under Verner's Law became identical with IE φ θ χ (bh dh gh), developing to Gmc. ƀ ð ȝ. These voiced spirants became stops, b d g, after nasals (24 b), and apparently also before n. Thus, before an accented n-suffix the three series p t k, φ θ χ (bh dh gh), and b d g became identical, namely, bn dn gn. Through assimilation of n to the preceding voiced stops, bb dd gg resulted, which became pp tt kk in Step IV. It is clear that this process occurred after Verner's Law and before the unvoicing of the voiced stops, unless we assume that the latter preceded Verner's Law, and that bb dd gg became voiceless through a second process of unvoicing; that is possible but unlikely.

    Instances:

    (The IE equations are in most cases mere formulae of reconstruction. This was essentially a Germanic process, whether the standard view be accepted in principle or not.)

    But we find Gk. 'ιπνός Go. aúhns 'oven' (ogn and ofn, ofen, OHG. ofan); Gk. τέκνον 'child' : OHG. degan 'warrior', in spite of the Greek root accent (the etymology is very uncertain). In the numerous n-derivatives of verbs (Gothic fourth weak class) we should expect suffix accent and therefore assimilation of n, but at least in Gothic, and partly in Norse, n is preserved: Go. -waknan, waknōda. Norse vakna, vaknada. Gothic has only two words with tt : atta 'father', skatts 'hoard', both uncertain as to etymology; two borrowed words with kk: sakkus 'sack' and smakka 'fig'; and no words with pp.

    After a long syllable these geminates were simplified:

    The Germanic gemination explains numerous double forms, such as schneiden and schnitzen, stossen and stutzen, Reh and Ricke (also Ziege and Zicke), given above. Simplification after a long syllable produces a different type of parallels. Gmc. knuppa- > Knopf 'button', but Gmc. knauppa-, knaupa- > knōp, Knauf 'hilt'. knübel 'knuckle' shows that the labial was not IE b, but either φ (bh), or p under Verner's Law. In High German, -p became f, but pp became pf. It is not always possible to decide whether such double forms are due to the Germanic or to the West Germanic Consonant Lengthening. Cf. 30.

    Probably very few cases of Germanic Consonant Lengthening actually go back to Indo-European forms. A few of these, like IE lugnós, may have established a pattern on which many new forms were modeled in the several Germanic languages. Once geminates had been established by assimilation, they could easily become the instrument of sound symbolism; through this, 'bildete schon das Urgermanische partielle Neuschopfungen durch Dehnung des wurzelauslautenden Konsonanten eines schon vorhandenen Wortes zur Bezeichnung der Intensität, ähnlich wie zu gleichem Zweck das Semitische durch Dehnung des mittleren der drei Wurzelkonsonanten.' (Loewe, GS 77 f.; similarly Braune, Ahd. Gramm. §95 Anm. 2.)

    Back to Table of Contents

    23.

    The Labiovelars.

    Back to Table of Contents

    a

    The most important development of these a sounds in other Indo-European languages have been given in 11. Their Germanic development is shown in the following table:

    The treatment of the labiovelars in Germanic is similar to that in Latin. Cf. for kw, sequor—secutus—socius; for gw, veniō < *gwṃjō, vivus < *gwīwos ; for ghw, ninguit—nīcs = nīx—nīvem.

    for gw, venid < *gwrnjd, vivus < *gwiwos ; for ghw, ninguit—nics = nil—nivem.

    Back to Table of Contents

    b

    The general principle is this: The velar element (k g gh) goes through the regular shift, including Verner's Law (indicated where needed by ´ for stressed, ` for unstressed syllable preceding). The labial element is retained under certain conditions, lost under others. But Gmc. ȝw is a 'phonetic interpolation' —an intermediate stage that must be assumed theoretically, though it is not preserved historically. It appears either as ȝ or as w; only after n (ŋ), Gothic and Norse have gw. This is exactly like the Latin treatment of IE χw (ghw, Brugmann's q*h): IE neiχw- (Gk. νείφει), ninχw- > nīg-, nīw-, ningw-.

    As in Latin, the labial element disappears before back vowels (IE u ū o ō—therefore secūlus, but sequitur; sequor is analogical formation for *secor, like equus for *ecos). It also disappears before consonants (cf. socius, secta), and doubtless too in final position (L. que = in Go. sah, nih; Go. saƕ, laiƕ) are analogical forms).

    Back to Table of Contents

    c

    Instances:

    Note: Gmc. ɧw can also come from IE ^kw, as Sk. śvētas, Go. ƕeits 'white', Sk. aśvas, L. equus, Go. aíha- 'horse' (iōr < *ekwos > *eɧwaz > *joɧaz > *johr, jōr). In a few instances, it apparently goes back to a plain velar followed by w, instead of a real labio-velar, in which the lip-rounding extends over both elements. Our transcription does not indicate this; in Brugmann's transcription, Go. afhapjan 'expire' — Lith. kvėpti 'breathe' would be *qwep-, while Go. hals is represented by *q*olso-. It does not seem expedient to complicate the whole system of transcription for the sake of a very few words.

    Through Verner's Law:

    Gmc. seɧwana- saɧw sǣɧwum seȝwana- -ena-
    Go. saiƕan saƕ sēƕum [saiƕans]
    ON siā sǫm [sēnn]
    OE sēon seah [sāwon] sewen
    OS sehan sah [sāwun] gisewan
    OHG sehan sah sāhum [gisehan] geseuuen (Notker)

    Go. siuns, siōn, sȳn 'sight' < *sekwnís > *seȝwnis.

    Note: The OE forms are Saxon; Anglian has sēgon ( = sǣgon), gesegen. According to the present standard view (cf . Streitberg, UG 116, 113; Hirt, HU 107 f.), we should expect g in the pret. pl., before Gmc. u, and in the past part. g, if the IE ending was -ono-, w, if it was -eno-. In either case, there are complications. Sievers, Btr. 5. 149, assumed that Gmc. ȝw from either source became w if the accent followed. This would simplify the explanation of the forms with w, but add to the difficulty presented by the forms with g. The view is now accepted by few (e.g. by Trautmann, Germanische Lautgesetze 57).

    IE kwetwōres (Sk. catvāras, Gk. τέσσαρες, L. quattuōr, Lith. keturì) 'four', Go. fidwōr; the other Gmc. forms go back to a *kwekwōres, with medial kw either through the influence of the initial, or by analogy with *penkwe 'five' : fiōrer, fēower, OS fiuwar, fior, OHG. feor, fior. The w of these forms must be due either to the initial labial, or to a suffix form -ēres instead of -ōres; Old Norse, however, has the neuter fiogor.

    Before consonants:

    IE sokwjós (L. socius) 'follower', secg, seggr 'companion, man'

    IE sekwti- 'sight', sicht, Eng. sight

    IE ghwren- (Gk. φρήν 'diaphragm, mind'), grunr 'suspicion'.

    In some instances, we find f ƀ for Gmc. ɧw ȝw. The fact is certain, but the exact conditions are not known; sometimes it seems to be due to assimilation to a preceding w or u. Cf. Noreen, UL 147, Hirt, HU 108 f.:

    IE wḷkwos 'wolf', Sk. vrkas, OSL. vehъkъ, Lith. vilkas: Go. wulfs, ulfr, wulf, OHG. wolf; with Verner's Law— IE wḷkwī (Sk. vṛkī), ylgr, wülpe 'she-wolf'

    IE kwekwóres 'four' (see above), Go. fidwōr, etc.; the f can also be explained through the influence of Go. fimf < *penkwe.

    IE ukwnos 'fire, stove' (Gk. 'ιπνός), Go. auhns, but ofn, OHG. ofan

    IE penkwe 'five' (Sk. pañca, Gk. πέντε), Go. fimf, fimm, OE OS fīf, OHG. fimf, finf (f may be assimilation to the initial labial, as inversely L. quinque shows assimilation of the initial to the medial consonant).

    Noreen, l.c., quotes fifty-odd instances, but most of them are quite uncertain.

    The Intermediate Shift (Step V)

    The term 'Intermediate' is not entirely apt. The change did not occur between the Germanic and the High German Shifts, but began in early Germanic times —probably during the first or second century A.D. —, and continued, in High German, contemporaneously with the earlier stages of the High German Shift. In a sense this Shift might be called 'Overlapping' rather than 'Intermediate'.

    Back to Table of Contents

    24

    Back to Table of Contents

    a

    The Germanic Voiced Spirants,ƀ ð ȝ, tended to become voiced stops, in accordance with the 'Fundamental Principle' (16). This is virtually the opposite of 'Lenition', a process of common occurrence in Celtic, Romance languages, and elsewhere (e.g. L. debēre > Fr. devoir, Span. [deƀer]), since lenition consists in the change of stops to spirants. But the two processes have at least this in common that a spirant is more apt to be retained or developed medially than initially.

    The development of ƀ ð ȝ to b d g was completed in Upper German, probably also in East Franconian (which by some scholars, e.g. Behaghel, is classed with Upper German). In the other Germanic dialects, the change appears in complete agreement, on the one hand with the chronology of the formation of the dialect units, and on the other with the place of articulation of these sounds and of their position in the words. That is to say: (1) In Gothic and Norse there is least evidence of these changes, more in Old English and Low German, still more in Middle German, while in Upper German the change is complete. (2) Dentals show the widest, velars the narrowest scope of the change. (3) The change appears sooner, and, therefore, more widely, in initial than in medial or final position.

    Back to Table of Contents

    b

    Germanic, Gothic, Norse:

    (1) Gmc. ƀ ð ȝ after nasals and in gemination are stops in all dialects; therefore, we ascribe this change to Primitive Germanic: Go. ON OE OS OHG. lamb, Go. gen. sg. lambis; Go. OE OS bindan, binda, OHG. bintan, Go. pret. band; Go. laggs = [laŋgs], langr, OE OS OHG. lang, Go. gen. sg. masc. laggis. If b d had been spirants, Gothic would have changed them to f þ when final as in hlaiƀishlaif, rēdanrairōþ, but not *lamf *banþ; for the velar, Gothic spelling would not indicate the difference; cf. 31. Instances of Gmc.i>ƀƀ ðð ȝȝ > bb dd gg have been given in 22.

    (2) Initial ƀ and ð appear as stops everywhere in Germanic, with the exception only of a few isolated forms in early Runic inscriptions, (ƀarutR = brȳtr 'breaks', Björketorp; ðohtriR = døtr 'daughters', Tune; cf. 18), for which, however, other explanations might be given. Cf. 18 and Noreen, Aisi. Gramm. 143: Go. bairan, bera, OE OS OHG. beran; Go. dauhtar, dōtter, dohtor, OS dohtar, OHG. tohter.

    (3) Gmc. ƀ ð became stops in Gothic after l r, probably also after other consonants, and in Norse ð became d after l: Go. haldan, halda, healdan, OS haldan, OHG. haltan; Go. gards, geard, OS gard, OHG. gart, but garþr; Go. halbis, gen. sg., halb, nom. sg. neut., OHG. halb, but halfr, healf, OS half.

    The treatment of Gmc. ȝ in Gothic and Norse must still be considered a moot question. It is most frequently assumed that it was a spirant medially, but a stop initially; for final Goth. g, cf. 31. But for Gothic the evidence is extremely slight (see especially Dieter, AD 194); for Norse there is no early evidence at all (cf. Noreen, Aisi. Gr. 143, Asw. Gr. 175), and the testimony of the modern languages is conflicting. It is best in keeping with the general trend to assume the following: In Gothic and early Norse, Gmc. ȝ was a spirant in all positions, except after nasals and in gemination; before back vowels and consonants it was the velar spirant ȝ (g in North German Tage), before front vowels, the palatal spirant (j): ȝ in Go. gards, garðr, j in Go. giban, gefa. This condition remained in Swedish and Norwegian, while in Danish initial ȝ became g. But it must be admitted that there is no direct evidence for either Gothic or Old Norse.

    Streitberg, GE (5th and 6th ed.) 63, claims stop pronunciation in all positions, not only for g, but even for b and d at the time of Wulfila; in his opinion, based on Sievers's Schallanalyse, 'fordert die Intonation fur die Zeit Wulfilas unzweideutig die stimmhaften Verschlusslaute'. In view of the alternation between final f þ and medial b d, the argument does not seem convincing.

    Back to Table of Contents

    c

    West Germanic:

    (1) Gmc. ð became d in all positions: fæder, OS fadar, OHG. fater; the change to ð in NE father, due to assimilation to the following r (as also in weather) took place around 1500.

    (2) Medial and final ƀ remained a spirant (generally voiceless when final) throughout the North: English, Frisian, Saxon, Low and Middle Franconian; it became a stop in the other German dialects: giefan, OFris. ieva (geva, jeva), OS geban, but OHG. geban. The Germanic spirant ƀ had been bilabial, and at least in Gothic and Old Saxon must doubtless still be considered as such. Later it became labio-dental [v] everywhere where it had not become a stop (bilabial in Go. sibun, labio-dental in E. seven). But since the end of the Middle Ages, medial b has been a bilabial spirant in Bavarian, Alsatian, and considerable parts of Middle German, particularly in Rhine Franconian.

    (3)The treatment of Gmc. ȝ is not altogether certain. For Old English, it is certain that medial and final ȝ were spirants, and initial ȝ is generally admitted to have been a spirant at least in West Saxon in the earliest period. This is clearly evident from the fact that it alliterates with the IE palatal spirant, as in the first line of Beowulf: gār < Gmc. ȝaiza- and gēar < Gmc. jǣra-, IE jēro-. But later in the Old English period, initial ȝ before consonants and back vowels (where it had velar articulation) became a stop, as in NE grass, good. Before front vowels (except those that came from the modified vowel ö and ü) it remained a spirant with palatal articulation : NE yard, yellow, yield. But this view is hard to reconcile with such forms as NE give, get, guest; since Kluge, Anz. z. Anglia 5. 83, the initial g in these words is generally ascribed to Scandinavian influence, but, as said above, there is no real evidence for stop pronunciation of ȝ- in Old Norse. It is more likely that the stop forms are of Anglian, the spirant forms of Frisio-Saxon origin.

    This is indicated primarily by spelling tradition. In West Saxon the spirantic (palatal) pronunciation of initial ȝ is shown by a following i or e, which are 'silent', having the function of diacritical marks: ȝiefan, ȝeaf, ȝēafon — [jevan, jæf, jǣfon]; in early MSS forms with eae = , instead of ea, occur, but apparently there developed a tradition against the writing of three consecutive vowels. The macron of ȝēafon, when used at all, is a mechanical transfer from the diphthong ēa < au. The ē is silent, and the a stands for æ. Anglian spelling, as far as it is not influenced by West Saxon tradition, has gefan, gæf, gǣfon, with initial stop'. (The distinction between two runes for Gmc. ȝ in the Ruthwell Cross inscription has nothing to do with this; it refers to palatal versus velar articulation, not to spirant versus stop.)

    Modern English dialects offer conflicting evidence. There seems to be a tendency in favor of the stop in the North and in favor of the spirant in the South, but dialect mixture has greatly interfered.

    If Anglian did have initial g for Gmc. ȝ, the shift may date back to continental times. This is suggested by the parallel development in the essentially Anglian territory east and north of the lower Elbe; see below. Apparently, the Saxon and Frisian element of Southern England brought the initial spirant from their continental homes, the Anglians the initial stop, and in both cases that early pronunciation has in a large measure prevailed to this day.

    Also in the continental West Germanic dialects, the evidence of spelling is not always sufficient to decide whether Gmc. ȝ remained a spirant or became a stop. Often the somewhat uncertain conditions of modern dialects are our only guide. In general, the distribution is as follows:

    Initial ȝ remained a spirant as far south as Middle Franconian, including the northern part of that dialect, but south of that district became a stop, so that its treatment is essentially the same as that of medial ƀ: L. jeven,geben. The spirant is usually palatal (j), but velar ȝ is found in some districts, especially in Low Franconian (Dutch). Medial ȝ remained a spirant (usually voiced, but partly voiceless) everywhere except in Upper German; cf. North German [la:ȝan li:jan]. However, a detached territory in the Northeast has the stop g for Gmc. ȝ both initially and medially: Initially, g is spoken in Schleswig Holstein, Mecklenburg-Schwerin, Pommern; medially, in Mecklenburg-Schwerin (Behaghel, GddS 212).

    This coincides with the initial stop in neighboring Danish. It is difficult to say whether there is any common cause for this. Might it have anything to do with the proximity of this territory to the Germanic homeland, that the phonetic trend continued somewhat longer in this district than in the older colonial lands? Perhaps it is not quite without importance that at least a part of this territory —chiefly Holstein —is identical with that Scatenauge Albiae fluvii ripa (= Scadinavia) from which the Langobards emigrated in the second half of the fourth century (Zeuss, Die Deutschen und die Nachbarstamme 471 f.). The consonantism of the Langobards had gone through the High German Shift (cf. Bruckner, Die Sprache der Langobarden 18 ff.; Behaghel, GddS 16), which is entirely in keeping with the theory presented in this book. However, initial þ probably did not become d, as in German, but t, as in Scandinavian.

    The High German Shift

    Back to Table of Contents

    25. Chronology and Spread.

    Back to Table of Contents

    a

    The changes described in the preceding paragraphs belong essentially to the period before the geographical separation of the Germanic ethnic unit. Aside from the incompleteness of the 'Intermediate' Shift, these are 'Germanic' changes, affecting more or less all Germanic dialects. After the formation of the East and West Germanic groups, approximately at the beginning of the Christian Era, all three groups possessed the following consonants, in addition to the nasals, liquids, and semi-vowels:

    ƀ ð ȝ; p t k; f þ ɧ; s z.

    The consonant changes after the separation show a marked divergence. In Gothic, and especially in Norse, new tendencies set in that led to consonant changes in new directions. In West Germanic, the Germanic Drift, that is, the physiological principle that had dominated the Germanic Consonant Shift, continued for a longer or shorter period, according to the time of emigration; cf. 17, end. The West Germanic expansion was directed mainly to the east and south, surrounding the East-Elbian homeland like the opening of a fan (cf. 6). In the north-western branch, comprising Frisian, Saxon, and Low Franconian, the old trend affected only the voiced spirants in the limited scope described in the preceding section. These dialects, therefore, differ from Gothic and Norse, as far as the Consonant Shift is concerned, only in the change of medial and final ð to d.

    If the trend of the Germanic Shift had continued unabated, the result would obviously have been:

    Gmc. p t k > Ger. f þ ɧ

    Gmc. ƀ ð ȝ > Ger. p t k (through b d g)

    But these changes took place only in the territory of relatively late expansion south of the 'Benrather Linie', that is, in the High German dialects, and even there with limitations and modifications. For this reason we speak of a 'High German' or 'Second Consonant Shift' in contrast to the 'First' or 'Germanic Shift'. In general, it followed the same trend, but in certain instances the phonetic direction was deflected, so as to lead to slightly different results. Moreover, in certain cases secondary developments counteracted changes that had actually taken place.

    The historical order and, therefore, the geographical spread of the Second Shift follows the Fundamental Principle stated in 17. In the Intermediate Shift, as shown above, the initial or medial (final) position of the consonants was an important factor; it may have been that, temporarily, in the First Shift too, but we have no historical evidence for this. In the Second Shift, particularly in the case of Gmc. p t k, it is of prime importance: the formation of spirants had a wider sway after vowels than initially, after consonants, and in gemination. Since in the Intermediate Shift the development progresses from spirants to stops, but in the Second Shift from stops to spirants, it follows logically that here initial position is unfavorable to the change, while there it was favorable to it.

    Back to Table of Contents

    b

    The maps on the following page (which include the area of later colonization east of the Elbe and Saale) outline the spread of the High German Consonant Shift; the shaded areas indicate the territories in which the respective changes took place.

    This distribution of the High German Shift is entirely in keeping with the chronological sequence of the West Germanic expansion outlined in connection with the Fundamental Principle of the Sound Shift (16). The Germanization of northwestern Germany took place soon after the separation of the East and North Germanic groups from the Central stock. Accordingly, we find that Frisian, Saxon, and Low Franconian continued the shift very slightly beyond the stage reached in the former groups. In Central Germany, the Roman limes halted the expansion for several centuries, and the Germans that had reached the middle Rhine before the time of Caesar received strong reinforcements from the central region during that time. This explains the great contrast between Low German and High German. The formation of the Upper German dialects is marked by the final occupation of Southwestern.

    Germany by the Alemannii, and of Vindelicia by the Baiuvarii, during the fifth and sixth centuries. The parallelism between the tribal consolidations and the stages of the Second Shift can be indi cated as follows:

    Occupation of the North Sea coast, last centuries B.C. —ð > d, ƀ- > b.

    Concentration at the limes, A.D. to 400—gradual spread of the shift of p t k, continuation of the 'Intermediate Shift'.

    Occupation of Upper Germany, 400 to about 500 — b > p, g > k; k- > kh.

    A detailed description of the process would go far beyond the limits of this book.

    Back to Table of Contents

    26. Instances and Limitations.

    The following instances represent the High German Shift in its extreme form, termed 'Streng-Althoch-deutsch' by Jacob Grimm. For the sake of convenience, post-vocalic position is indicated by -p -t -k, while p- t- k- indicate initial position, position after consonants, and gemination.

    (1) Voiceless stops after vowels become voiceless spirants: -p -t -k > ff zz hh. These new spirants are not identical with Gmc. f þ ɧ, these had in the course of time become lenes, while the new ones were fortes. This is generally indicated by double spelling, but after long vowels and in final position simple consonants are commonly used: Go. skipis (gen. sg.), skip, slēpan — OHG. skiffes, skif, slāfan (slāffan) Go. itan, ētum, þata — OHG. ezzan, āzum, daz (thaz) Go. mikils, bdka, ik—OHG mihhil, michil, puoh, ih.

    ff (f) was probably labio-dental from the very beginning, since this articulation is better adapted to fortis pronunciation than the bilabial one.

    zz (z) is used consistently in OHG and MHG MSS (the Isidor MS uses zss, zs); grammatical works generally use z, to distinguish the spirant from the affricate z = ts. Its pronunciation is not known, but it was clearly distinguished from s. Perhaps it was originally a slit spirant, a fortis þ, which changed to a rill spirant, s, towards the end of the MHG time. In most modern High German dialects, the distinction is merely one between fortis and lenis: lassenlesen; voiced -s- is a borrowing from Low German.

    For hh (h), ch is generally used since the middle of the ninth century. Like ȝ, it is assimilated to the preceding vowel, leading to the distinction between the ich- and the ach-sound. Alemannian and a part of Southern Bavarian have only the ach-sound.

    2) Voiceless stops initially, after consonants, and in gemination change to the corresponding affricates: p- t- k- > pf ts (z) kh (kch):

    These changes are carried through to the following extent:

    (a) The shift of -p -t -k and t- covers the whole High German territory, but Middle Franconian preserves dat, it, wat, the ending -et, and, partly, the prepositiup; ('Restwörter' cf. 17 d). Also d > t may be termed general High German, but the voiceless stop was (and is) a lenis in Middle and Rhine Franconian, a fortis (originally) in East Franconian and Upper German.

    (b) p- > pf belongs essentially to East Franconian and Upper German; in Middle Franconian the change did not take place at all, in Rhine Franconian only after l and r (lpf, rpf, later generally lf, rf) : MFr. RFr. Pund, Palz, MFr. helpen.

    (c) k- > is general Upper German in early OHG times, but now the affricate is found only in some southern dialects of Switzerland; in other Swiss dialects and in the Tyrol the spirant ch is pronounced: chalt.

    (d) The shift of b g to p k belongs to Upper German of the earlier period; the details are not of importance for this brief treatment.

    B. Other Consonant Changes

    Back to Table of Contents

    27.

    Back to Table of Contents

    a

    The Germanic Voiceless Spirants, f þ ɧ, had partly become voiced under the conditions of Verner's Law. Originally, they were doubtless fortes, since they had developed in the same way as the later High German spirants, but since prehistoric times they showed a progressive tendency towards a weakening in articulation. This is a natural trend, since the relatively unchecked current of expiration gradually lessens the resistance of the muscles of the lips or tongue, and then, in turn, loses its own force. Their later development in the several Germanic languages varied according to their place of articulation and their position.

    The dental þ, followed the tendency of the Consonant Shift consistently. Unless it was preserved as a voiceless spirant (see below), it became ð, later in part d. The labial f, was perhaps originally bilabial, but there is no certain evidence for this. (Assimilations like Go. OHG. fimf < *penkwe prove nothing. The m may have been labio-dental itself; besides, such pronunciations are common even in modern German, which certainly has labio-dental f.) At any rate, its clearness of articulation was enhanced by labio-dental articulation, which we now find in all Germanic languages; in certain positions and in certain languages it became voiced. The velar, for which this book uses the letter ɧ, (modified h, since h is the usual spelling in all Germanic languages) had the least stability: being articulated by the relatively inert velum and back of the tongue, it lacked the articulatory intensity that would have favored a further shift; under Verner's Law it had become ȝ at a time when it was still pronounced with considerable friction although a lenis. Where it remained voiceless, its physiological character led to further relaxation, resulting in its reduction to actual h, the glottal (instead of velar) spirant, and sometimes in complete disappearance. The following is in part a summing-up of statements occurring in former sections.

    Back to Table of Contents

    b

    Gmc. þ was preserved in Gothic in all positions: þu, wairþan, brōþar, warþ. In Old Norse and Old English it remained þ initially, but became ð in voiced surroundings: þing—brōðor, þingbrōðor; spelling, however, does not indicate this, since the letters þ and ð are used interchangeably in these two languages. In Old Saxon and Old High German we find þ (mostly spelled th) in the earliest records in all positions, but gradually this changed to ð and soon to d, first medially and in habitually unaccented words, e.g., in pronouns and in the definite article. In Bavarian, the process began as early as the eighth century. It gradually spread north, reaching the Low German territory during the late Middle Ages. In Middle English, perhaps even in Old English, þ- in pronominal forms and similar words of little semantic function became ð, and an analogous process took place in Danish, Swedish, and Norwegian where, however, stops resulted: Sw. ting, tānka, but du, De. English and Icelandic preserved þ initially, English also in strictly final1 position (cf. clothclothe; in the latter word th was medial in Middle English).

    Back to Table of Contents

    c

    Gmc. f was also preserved in Gothic in all positions: fadar, afar, uf. Like þ, it became voiced in voiced surroundings in Norse and English, thus coinciding with IE bh and `p. In Low German (Old Saxon), too, it became voiced. The usual spelling was ƀ, but we also find u: wulƀos, fiui. In High German it generally remained voiceless, but has been a lenis since OHG times, as its frequent rendering by u v in medial position indicates: nevo, nefor, hevig.

    Back to Table of Contents

    d

    Gmc. ɧ was clearly the ach-/ich-sound, as is indicated by Latin spellings like Cherusci, Chatti, Gk. Χέρουσκοι, Χάττοι. In all Germanic languages it is preserved initially as h, but in other positions it must be considered a velar (or palatal) spirant wherever preserved at all : h in Go. haban, hafa, etc.; [χ] (or [ç]) in Go. nahts, raihts, taih. However, final h in Gothic, while still a velar spirant, must have been decidedly a lenis, while final ɧ, which became unvoiced like final ð and ƀ, was a fortis: dag, dags probably had a velar spirant with stronger friction than taih, comparable to the difference in the articulation of ch in Bach and nach. In Norse, h disappeared everywhere except initially, in Old English it disappeared between voiced sounds: Go. himins, ƕa: himinn, huat, heofon, hwæt; Go. teihan, saiƕan, slahan, aƕa, filhan: tiā, siā, slā, ā, fela, tēon, sēon, slēan, ēa, feolan. In both Low and High German final and intervocalic h is consistently preserved in the older sources, but gradually disappeared, first in Bavarian; cf. sehen, sah = [ze:ən, za:]. Before consonants it remained a velar or palatal spirant: Nacht, Sicht. —Initially it disappeared before l r n w in Norse as well as in West Germanic; only English, at least American English, has preserved hw: Go. hlahjan, hlǣja, hliehhan, OHG. (h)lahhan— Sw. le, E. laugh, Ger. lachen; Go. hneiwan, hniga, OE OS OHG. hnīgan; neigen; Go. hrains, hreinn, OS hrēni, OHG. (h)reini: rein; Go. ƕa, hvat, hwæt, OS hwat, OHG. hwaz: Sw. hvad = [vad], E. what - Brit. [wɔt], L. wat, was.

    Back to Table of Contents

    28. Rhotacism

    IE s was preserved intact in Germanic where it had not changed to z according to Verner's Law. This z remained in Gothic, but in North and West Germanic it was intensified to r, as in Latin (genera < *genezā). This is called Rhotacism from rho, the Greek name of the letter r. In earliest Norse this r is distinct from old r: It is expressed by a different rune (transcribed by ʀ in our grammars) and causes i-mutation (Go. kas 'vessel', ker). Early Norse loan words in Finnic still show s (rengas, kuningas). In later Norse and in West Germanic there is no distinction between old and new r. —For s in final syllables, see 49 d.

    Instances:

    Note: Go. huzd goes back to IE kudh-tó- > kud-dhó-, kudˢdhó-, through 'Bartholomā's Law'; it is related to Gk. κύσθος 'concealed place, female organ', L. custos 'guardian': similarly >*ghədh-tó-, L. hasta, Go. gazds, OS gard, OHG. gart 'spike'. IE z goes back to s before a voiced stop. In such cases, through regular consonant shift, Gmc. sp st sk result, as in *ni-zdo-, L. nīdus, OHG. nest, *o-zdo-, Go. Go. zm, OHG. ast.

    For Gmc. zm, see below.

    German Grammar

    Back to Table of Contents

    29. Consonant Groups

    (1) s-Combinations.

    (a)

    The IE group tt remained as such only in Indic: IE sed-to- > setto- 'seated' > Sk. satta-. In Iranian, Slavic, Greek it developed to st: Gk. 'α-ιστος' 'unknown' < IE ṇ-wid-to-. In Italic, Celtic, Germanic the result was ss: L. sessus, ON OE OS sess 'seat'; Go. wissa, ON vissa, OE wisse, OS OHG wissa 'I knew' < IE wid-tām; Go. OHG missa- 'wrong' < IE mid-to- (L. mitto, missus, OE OS mīþan, OHG mīdan 'omit'). After a long vowel, ss is simplified to s; IE weid-to-, Go. un-weis, ON vīs- (nom. sg. masc. vīss), OE OHG wīs 'wise'; IE ēdtom (past part. of edō, Go. itan), OE ǣs, OHG ās 'carcass'. It is generally assumed that the transition to st ss took place after a sibilant glide had developed between the two t's: t*t; either the first t, or both t's were assimilated to this. Apparently also IE ts resulted in Gmc. ss s, but evidence for this is very scant: Go. missō 'mutually', ON ȳ-miss 'alternately' may go back to IE mit-sām (Sk. adv. mithas 'mutually'), ON eisa 'embers' to IE əidh-s, əits- (Gk. αιθος).

    (b)

    Inversely, a t-glide developed in Germanic and Slavic between s and r. IE sreu-/srou- 'flow', Sk. srávati, Gk. ρεω, ON straumr, OE strēam, OS OHG strōmo-strovъ 'island'; IE ausro-/usro- 'bright', Sk. uṣrá, L. aurōra, Lith. aušra 'morning glow', OE ēastron, OHG ōstarun 'spring festival, Easter'; IE swesr-, Sk. svasár-, L. soror, Go. swistar, OE swester, OS OHG swester (t developed in those cases that had originally suffix with zero grade).

    b (2) Nasals and Liquids show hardly any independent sound changes (for final m, see 49 d), but they are very apt to assimilate other sounds, or to be assimilated to them. The following assimilations are the most important:

    (a)

    The Germanic Consonant Lengthening, pn' bn' bhn' > pp; see 22. (b)

    Likewise, ln > ll: IE pḷnós, Lith. pílnas, Sk. -pṛṇa-, OSl. plъnъ, Go. fulls, ON fullr, OE OS ful, OHG fol; IE peln-, Gk. πέλλα, L. pellis, Go. þrūtsfill 'leprosy', ON fiall, OE fell, OHG fel (gen. felles).

    (c)

    nw > nn: Sk. riṇváti 'makes flow', Go. OS OHG rinnan 'flow'; L. minuō 'diminish', Go. minniza 'less'; L. tenuis, ON þunnr, OE þynne, OHG dunni 'thin'.

    (d)

    n-stop-n > nn: IE sent-no- 'go' (L. sentiō, Go. sinþs 'walk'; OE sīþian < IE sent-jo-) —OHG sinnan 'go, consider'.

    (e)

    zm > mm, zl > ll, ðl > ll: Sk. tasmāi 'him' (dat. sg.), Go. þamma (simplified through unstress in OHG demu, demo; likewise dat. pl. ending IE -mis > Gmc. -miz mz mm m; see 49 d); IE es-mi 'am', Sk. ásmi, Gk. *εσμι, ειμι, Go. im < *imm. —ON knosa, OE cnyssan, OHG chnussen 'strike'; ON knylla, OE cnyllan 'strike' < *knuz-la-. — IE stədhlo-, L. stabulum, ON stallr, OE steall, OHG stal (gen. sg. stalles) (but IE stətlo- > OE staþol, OHG stadel 'shed'); IE mə-tlo- 'meet', with suffix accent Gmc. maðla-, Ger.-L. mallus 'place of court', mallāre 'sue', Theotmalli 'Detmold', but with stem accent, Gmc. maþla-, Go. maþl 'place of meeting, ON *mahla- māl, OE mæþl, OHG mahal, madal-. c

    (f)

    n (ŋ) before ɧ disappears in Primitive Germanic; in Norse, the nasal also disappears before s and f, and in the Anglo-Frisian group (OE Fris. OS) before s f þ; a preceding short vowel is lengthened ('Compensatory Lengthening'); a is always lengthened to ō in OE, sometimes in ON OFris. and OS:

    • Gmc. faŋɧana-, Go. fāhan, ON , OE fōn, OFris. , OS OHG fāhan
    • Gmc. þaŋɧta-, pret. of þaŋkjana- 'think', Go. þāhta, ON þātta, OE þōhte, OS thāhta, OHG dāhta
    • Gmc. þeŋɧana- 'thrive', Go. -þeihan, OE þēon, OHG -dīhan
    • Gmc. þuŋɧta-, pret. of þuŋɧjana- 'think, seem', Go. þūhta, ON þōtta, OE þūhte, OS thūhta, OHG dūhta
    • E. think, OE þyncean, belongs to this verb, but E. thought, OE þōhte, belongs to Gmc. paŋkjana-, OE þencean; thus E. thinkthought corresponds to NHG dünkendachte; cf. methinksmich dünkt.
    • OHG gans, ON gǫs, OE *gās, gōs
    • Go. OHG uns, ON ōs, OE ūs, OFris. OS ūs
    • OHG wunsken 'wish', ON øskia, OE wȳscan
    • Gmc. tumft- 'foundation wall' (L. domus, Gk. δόμος) > ON tōft, tōpt
    • Go. OHG fimf, OE OFris. OS fīf; instead of ON *fīf we have fimm, formed in analogy with the ordinal fimte and fimtān 'fifteen', in which f had been lost between two consonants.
    • Go. kunþs 'known', OHG kund, OE cūþ, OFris. OS kūth
    • Go. anþar, OHG andar, ON annarr ( > nn), OE ōþer, OFris. ōther, OS āðar, ōthar (but also andar —a HG borrowing).

    The similar forms ON ǫþrom, OE ōþrum, dat. sg. masc. of 'other', represent very different phonetic processes; in OE, n before þ disappeared with the regular compensatory lengthening to ō; in Norse, the group became nn (nom. sg annarr), and this became ð(þ) before r, as in kuþr - Go. kumþs (> *kunnr > kuþr).

    Some other assimilations are obvious, such as md > nd : ON sund 'swimming' (noun), OE OS OHG swimman, ON symia.

    d

    For Go. þliuhan as against ON flȳia, OE flēon, OFris. fliā, OS OHG fliohan 'flee' it is generally claimed that Gothic has retained the original initial, while the other dialects have dissimilated the dentals þ and l. But there can really not be any doubt that Zupitza (Germ. Gutt. 131) is right in accepting Gmc. fl- as original and considering Go. þl- an assimilation. The verb is related to ON fliūga, OE flēogan, OHG fliogan, and is an extension of IE pleu- by a k-determinant. The simple root occurs in Gk. πλέϝω 'float, swim', OSl. pluti, Lith. plauti, Sk. plavatē, L. pluit 'rains', OHG flouwen 'rinse', Go. flōdus 'flood', OHG fluot, etc. With d-extension it appears in ON fliōta, OE flēotan, OS fliotan, OHG fliozzan 'flow'. Its primary meaning was probably 'floating, even motion, through water, air, or on land'. OHG fliogan etc. must be considered an aorist present; the stem vowel was leveled in accordance with the class pattern (instead of *fluȝan > *flogan), but Verner's Law was preserved. The same principle applies to Go. þlahsjan 'frighten' and -þlaihan 'exhort', both of which belong to the family of L. plangō. For Go. forms with fl- (flahta, -flaugjan, flauts, flōdus, flōkan) see Nordmeyer, Language 11. 216 ff.

    e There are some sporadic dissimilations, for which no definite formula can be given. The most frequent cases are various types of dissimilation between m and n: Gk. στόμα 'mouth', IE stomn-, stemn- : Go. stibna 'voice', OFris. stifne— OE stemm, OS stemna, OHG stimna 'Stimme'; IE nomn- 'name', ON nafn, Go. namō, gen. namnis; IE kemn- 'heaven', OE heofon, OS hevan —Go. himins (while in ON and OE the labial nasal is replaced by a labial spirant, in OHG himil the dental nasal is replaced by a dental liquid).

    Back to Table of Contents

    30. The West Germanic Consonant Lengthening

    As shown in 22, n was, in Primitive Germanic, under certain accent conditions assimilated to a preceding consonant, resulting in the geminates Gmc. pp tt kk. A similar, but by no means identical process took place in West Germanic at a later time: Before semi-vowels and liquids (j w l r) consonants were lengthened, but the assimilation of j w was gradual, and l r were not assimilated. Phonetically the process is not entirely clear, but is apparently connected with a difference in syllable division: In a word like Go. hal-dan, l belongs clearly to the first syllable; but ð in bidjan belongs to the second syllable as well as the first; this may have led to a lengthening of articulation. The type bidjan, with a j-suffix, is by far the most frequent. The palatal spirant palatalizes the preceding consonant, and a palatal consonant is in its very nature a long consonant (cf. Bremer, Deutsche Phonetik 48; Prokosch, Sounds and History of the German Language 25 f.). The habit of articulation that had developed in this type may then have been transferred to the similar types where other sonorous consonants followed.

    Instances:

      j:

    • Go. bidjan, ON biþia— OE biddan, OS biddian, OHG bitten
    • Go. sitan, ON sitia--OE sittan, OS sittian, OHG sizzen
    • Go. skapjan, ON skepia, OE scieppan, OHG scepfen
    • Go. sibja, OE sibb, OS sibbia, OHG sippea, sippa.

    g k after a short vowel were lengthened in Norse too:

    • Go. ligan, ON liggia, OE licgan, OS liggian, OHG liggen UG licken Go. hugjan, ON hyggia, OE hycgan, OS huggian, OHG huggen (UG hucken) Gmc. bakja- 'brook', ON bekkr, OE bec(c); OHG bah < *baki-.
    • ON kk is generally leveled to k through analogy with other forms of the same word, e.g. Go. wakjan, ON vekia (trans.; intr. vaka, pret. vakþa), OE wacian < *wakajan, OS wekkian, OHG wecchen.

    r is generally not lengthened: Go. nasjan, OE OS nerian, OHG neren, but also nerren

    OHG skara 'troup', *skarjo > skerjo 'troup leader', NHG Scherge 'bailiff'. The resulting long consonant is shortened after a long syllable: Go. laidjan 'lead', ON leiþa, OE lǣdan, OS lēdian, OHG leiten (early UG leitten). w was more often vocalized to u o, but there are a few cases of WGmc. lengthening of labio-velars before w: Go. naqaþs 'naked', OHG nackot, ON nokkueþr, but without lengthening ON nakenn, OE nacod, OHG nahhut Go. aqizi 'axe', OHG ackus (but also ahhus) Go. alva, NHG -ach in Salzach, but a in Fulda; the Monsee-Wien Fragments of OHG have kisāhhun = Go. -sēƕun, nāhhitun = Go. nēƕidun. Before l r a vowel is inserted subsequent to the lengthening: l: OSl. (j)ablъko 'apple', OE æppel, ON eple, OHG apful (and afful) Go. leitils, ON lītell, OE lȳtel (simplified after long vowel), OS luttil, OHG luzzil r: Go. baitrs 'bitter', ON bitr, OE bittor, OS OHG bittar (tr- does not go through the HG Shift) Go. akrs, ON akr, OE æcer, without lengthening, OS akkar, OHG ackar OSl. dobrъ 'good', ON dapr 'sad', E. dapper, OHG tapfar 'weighty'

    n is a frequent noun suffix; through different vowel grades in the suffix there arise many parallel forms; if n follows the root without a vowel, lengthening takes place. A great deal of leveling has largely obscured the original conditions. In many words the alternation is still clear, but it is not always possible to separate this process from the Gmc. consonant lengthening (22), which within paradigms may have been delayed by analogical retention of older forms: Gmc. knab- (IE gnə-/gnō-bh-), as an n-stem, could show the suffix grades -ōn-, -ēn-, -n-, -ō-; the latter form gave OE cnafa (NE knave), OHG knabo 'Knabe'; forms with zero grade yielded OHG knappo 'Knappe' ; in either form, the simple or lengthened consonant was leveled through the whole paradigm. Similar Rabe—Rappe; E. drop —OHG tropfo (Gmc. -p-on-, -p-n-).

    Back to Table of Contents

    31. Secondary Developments

    The Consonant Shift had the same trend in all dialects, but it differed in scope, as shown in 18-26. Beyond this, the development of the consonants presents great variations.

    Gothic in the form in which we chiefly know it, that of Wulfila's time (4th century), had deviated very little from Germanic consonantism. It shows a preference for voiceless rather than voiced spirants: Verner's Law is carried through very incompletely; final voiced spirants (including position before -s, -t) are unvoiced: hlaibis, gōdis, maiza, dagis —hlaif(s), gōþ(s), mais, dag(s) (voiceless in spite of spelling; cf. 24b); giban —gaf, gaft. —Assimilations are comparatively rare; the doubling of consonants has not progressed beyond the Gmc. conditions; there is no Gothic evidence for the Gmc. lengthening of stops.

    Back to Table of Contents

    Gothic beyond Wulfila is virtually unknown. Neither the smaller documents, nor proper names preserved in historians, nor the fragments of Crimean Gothic offer adequate evidence for later consonant developments. Busbeck's spelling (cf. 5,Bibliography) seems to point to some changes that remind one of the second consonant shift, such as plut 'sanguis', tag 'dies', bruder 'frater', but the evidence is too contradictory to allow any clear conclusions. Aside from the unreliable spelling, there exists the possibility of dialect mixture; even High German influence is not quite excluded.

    Back to Table of Contents

    b

    Norse has greatly modified the Germanic consonant system. Its most striking features are the voicing, assimilation, or dropping of medial or final voiceless spirants and the extreme frequency of double consonants, which are in most cases the result of assimilation. Cf. Go. drigkan, þugkjan, anþar, lagjan, ON drekka, þykkia, annarr, leggia. Voiced stops that were already final in Primitive Norse, became voiceless: *band > *bant > batt. Whatever the reason, it is a fact that in its final appearance Norse consonantism shows a strange resemblance to Finnish consonantism.

    Back to Table of Contents

    c

    English shows in the early period two clearly distinct consonant systems. The northern, Anglian, section hardly differs from continental Anglian: Initial Gmc. ȝ is a stop, Gmc. k and g are not palatalized (see below). The southern territory, which is essentially Frisian (partly Saxon), retains in part an earlier consonant stage, in so far as ȝ, remains a spirant in all positions (except after n and in gemination; initially only before front vowels), in part it develops a new consonant trend that is common in Romance and Slavic, but among the Germanic languages occurs only in Frisian proper, in Southern English, and to a slight extent in later Scandinavian. This is 'Palatalization' , a term that, unfortunately, is used with a variety of meanings. In the sense that it has here, it does not refer to variation in the place of articulation of Gmc. ɧ ȝ in accordance with neighboring vowels (ich and ach, j and ȝ), but to the change of the stops, k g to palatal or dental affricates, č ts ds. While conditions in English and Frisian are not quite identical, they are sufficiently similar to make us assume that the process was either completed in continental times, or, at least, that k g before front vowels had become so decidedly palatal at the time of the Germanic settlement of Southern England that the actual 'assibilation' —the change to the affricates mentioned —was nearly inevitable : OE cirice, OFris. tzerke (ziurke) 'church' (Anglian kirk); OE cytel, OFris. szetel 'kettle' (Anglian; Go. katils); OFris. tsyse, OE cīese 'cheese'; OE drencean, OFris. drentza 'water' (cf. E. drench); OE licgean, OFris. lidza 'lie'; OE lǣce, OFris. letza 'leech, physician'; OE sprǣc, OFris. spreze 'speech'.

    Back to Table of Contents

    d

    Old Saxon virtually retained the West Germanic consonant system; it is still essentially intact in its present-day development, and equally so in Low Franconian, including Dutch. For the change of dental spirants to stops, cf. 24c.

    Back to Table of Contents

    e

    Old High German is distinguished from the other Germanic dialects chiefly by the Second Shift, but later consonant changes partly restored former conditions. Rhine Franconian, East Franconian, and Bavarian changed intervocalic b to bilabial v [b]. The Upper German fortes p t k became, in general, lenes; Upper German reverted to k in most of the territory.

    The consonantism of Standard New High German is, in a sense, an artificial structure. As to spelling, it is based on the East Franconian dialect, since its most important home, the East Middle German dialect, is primarily a continuation of East Franconian. But this East Middle German form of the language is interpreted in North German (in fact, almost international) sound values. Thus, German d is voiced in the North, voiceless in the South, but Standard German ('Bühnenaussprache') requires the northern pronunciation. German t is the South German equivalent of North German d (HG tun, LG don), and in its home is pronounced voiceless, mostly as a lenis, partly as a fortis, but never aspirated; still, the 'standard' requires the aspirated pronunciation, which in North German occurs only in those words which in South German have z, not t (HG zehn, LG ten). Medial b is an entirely artificial introduction; Gmc. -b- ia a voiceless lenis stop in Alemannian and in the 'Anglian' district east of the Lower Elbe, and a labio-dental or bilabial spirant everywhere else—but Standard German requires a voiced stop. k- is practically the only stop that has the same value in Low and High German, partly due to the retrogressive development of kh > k in Upper German.

    C. The Semi-Vowels

    Back to Table of Contents

    32. Articulation

    j and w may be defined as vowels (i u) in conso-antic function. Brugmann, accordingly, uses the characters for consonantic IE i u. In Go. stigum, i is purely a vowel; in Go. staig it is a diphthongal glide which may equally well be designated as a vowel or as a consonant; in IE dvojō-, Sk. dvayōs 'of two' it is clearly a consonant as to function, and it is inclined to become a consonant in articulation as well: ON tueggia (see below). The physiological distinction between vowels and consonants is relative. With a vowel, the current of breath is not impeded in its passage through the mouth; with a consonant, there is an obstruction in the median line of the mouth, be it complete (stops) or partial (spirants). But in the case of the 'high' vowels, i u, the back of the tongue is so close to the roof of the mouth that a slight raising is apt to transform them into spirants, or even stops. It is impossible to draw a definite line between vocalic i u and spirantic j w. English y and South German j are relatively wide and therefore more nearly vocalic; North German j and palatal intervocalic g are real spirants (Jahr, liegen). In Gmc. words borrowed by Romance languages at various times during the Middle Ages, initial w became a stop: LFr. wardon —Late L. guardāre, Fr. guarder; E. war— Fr. guerre; Ger. Welfen, Waiblingen — Ital. Guelfi, Ghibellini.

    Back to Table of Contents

    33. Germanic Developments

    a

    Initial j, w remained unchanged in most IE languages; in Greek, j- appears either as h (') or as ζ; the reasons for the difference are unknown. w is preserved as ϝ (Digamma) in many dialects, but had disappeared in (classical) Attic : Sk. yūgám, Go. jus —Gk. υμεισ 'you'; Sk. yugam, L. iugum, Go. juk —Gk. ζυγόν'yoke'; Go. jēr 'year', Czech jaro 'spring'—Gk. ωρα 'season' (L. hora is borrowed from this), Gk. Ηρα 'goddess of the seasons'; Sk. dyāus 'sky', Gk. Ζεύσ, L. Jupiter, Diēspiter, ON Tyr, OHG Ziu Zio (from *dejwos; the Sk. Gk. L. forms from *djēws); verb suffix -ejo-: Go. nasjan, OE nerigean, OHG nerian (nerren). —Gk. (ϝ)οικοσ 'house', L. vicus, Go. weihs 'town'; Gk. (ϝ)οιδα, Sk. vēda, L. vīdī, Go. wait 'I know'; Sk. vidhávā, L. vidua, Go. widuwō 'widow'.

    Initial j disappeared in Norse: Go. jēr, juk, ON ār, ok; initial w before l r disappeared (gradually) in NWGmc.; in Norse it also disappeared before rounded vowels: Go. anda-wleizn ON and-lit, OHG ant-luzzi 'face'; Go. wrikan, ON reka, OHG rehhan 'punish, revenge'; Go. waurþum, waurþans < Gmc. wurðum, wurðana-, ON urþom, orþenn.

    In NWGmc. we find sporadic loss of w after consonants: OE hwōsta, OHG huosto 'cough'; OE swēte < *swoti, OHG suozi 'sweet'. Between consonants it disappeared everywhere: Lith. leñgvas, Go. leihts 'light'.— For w of labio-velars, cf. 23.

    Back to Table of Contents

    b

    Owing to their semi-vocalic character, j and w in medial and final position are apt to alternate with i and u. The original conditions are largely obscured by secondary changes, but Gothic seems to have preserved the Primitive Germanic forms:

    (a) When j w became final through the loss of the original final vowel, they appeared as i u: Go. kuni, hari, hairdi (acc. sg. of jo-stems), but kunjis, harjis (gen. sg., < *kun-ji-za, *har-jv-za; for hairdeis, see below); kniu, triu (nom. acc. sg. neut.), but kniwis, triwis. Similarly hardus, < *hardwaz, sunjus (nom. pl. < *sun-iw-iz), but suniwe; cf. 49 d.

    The treatment of Gmc. -ji- in endings is of special importance. Gothic shows -ji- if the stem syllable has a short vowel and a consonant, or a long vowel without a consonant, but ī (spelled ei) if it has a long vowel and a consonant or is dissyllabic: satjis (W V, 2 sg.), harjis (gen. sg.), stōjis (2 sg.), but sōkeis (2 sg.), mikieis (2 sg.), hairdeis (gen. sg.). This is due to a difference in syllabification. In the former type, the syllable division is between stem and ending: sat-jis, har-jis, stō-jis. In the latter type, the final consonant of the stem belongs to the next syllable: sō-keis, miki-leis, hair-deis, and interco>nsonantic -ji- = ii was contracted to ī. ON hirþis (hirþes), but niþs, sǣker, but setr, show a further shortening; cf. 49 d.

    From the Gmc. point of view the types harjis, hairdeis on the one hand, and satjis, sōkeis on the other, are phonetically equivalent. The nouns, however, have the suffix IE je/jo (cf . L. medius, Go. midjis), while in the verbs two types have coalesced; some are je/jo- presents like L. speciō, Sk. páśyati 'he sees', Lith. slójuo-s 'I stand up' (L. sto < *stojo) : Go. bidjis, stōjis, sōkeis; but the great majority (verbs of Class I W) are causatives in IE eje/ejo (cf. 54 d, e, f, g) with early reduction to je/jo : satjis, lagjis, frawardeis (cf . Sk. vartáyati 'he turns', trans.).

    The difference in treatment according to the character of the preceding syllable is not restricted to Germanic, but appears also in Indo-Iranian, Latin (capis, but audis), and at least in traces in Greek and Baltic. It may have been Indo-European; this view was first expressed by Sievers (Btr. 5. 129 f.; it is frequently referred to as 'Sievers' Law') and confirmed and extended by F. Edgerton, Language 10. 235 ff. However, the identical development of Gmc. ji < IE je and Gmc. ji < IE eje rather seems to suggest independent development in the several languages.

    Back to Table of Contents

    c

    Holtzmann's Law.

    Intervocalic j w after short vowels show in many words strengthened articulation; we may represent the Germanic forms as jj ww (Braune, Btr. 9. 545). In West Germanic, the first part of the lengthened semi-vowel forms a diphthong with the preceding short vowel, but in Gothic and Norse it is narrowed to a stop. For ww we find ggw in both dialects; for jj Gothic has the spelling ddj, Norse ggi (ggj), but originally at least the pronunciation was probably the same, namely, a palatal stop followed by a spirantic glide (similar to gy in Magyar): j: Sk. dváyōs 'of two'—Go. tweddjē, ON tueggia: OHG (Isidor) zweiio Sk. priyā 'wife'— ON Frigg: OHG Frīa < *frijja (goddess) Gk. φόν—Go. *addi, Crimean Go. ada (probably pl.), ON egg: OE æg (pron æy; NE egg is a Norse loan-word), OHG ei. w: IE drewā —Go. triggwa 'alliance', ON tryggvar (pl.) 'trust': OHG triuwa, treuwa 'faith' IE ghləwo- (Gk. χλωρός 'light green') —Go. glaggwo 'accurately', ON gloggr 'clever': OE glēaw, OHG glouwēr 'clever' IE bhlewo- (bhləwo-, L. flāvus?)— Go. bliggwan: OHG bliuwan 'beat'.

    Note: Go. ggw in such words is not ŋgw, as in siggwan; bliggwan ia a strong verb of the second, siggwan of the third class.

    But there is no lengthening in Go. ais (aiz), OHG ēr, ON eir < *ajaz, Sk. ayas 'bronze'; Go. bajōþs 'both'; Go. triwis, kniwis (see above). Sometimes we have parallel forms with and without lengthening, similar to Grammatical Change: ON Frigg, but Go. frijōn 'love'; ON hoggua 'strike, cut', but Go. hawi'hay'.

    The difference is generally ascribed to IE accent, but the exact conditions are a moot question. Holtzmann, Ad. Gr. 1. 109, assumed lengthening if the accent preceded; cf. Kluge, Urg. 3. 75; QF 32. 127. Lately, strong arguments have been given for suffix accent, especially by Mikkola, Streitberg-Festgabe 267, and Trautmann, Germ. Lautgesetze 40 (with complete material). Also Hirt, HU 1. 113, favors this view. Leveling has so greatly obscured the original conditions that historical certainty can hardly be obtained. Phonetic probability would rather seem to point to root accent as the cause of this strengthening of articulation; cf. Prokosch, JEGPh 11. 7.

    In a few cases we find g instead of w in West Germanio too: L. iuventus —OHG jugund, OE geogoþ; L. novem —OS nigun. Probably the following u caused dissimilation in such forms.

    For intervocalic w > k, see Hirt, HU 1. 114.

    In Norse and West Germanic, with the exception of English, w became a true spirant, before 1000 in Norse, at the end of the Middle Ages in the German dialects. It is bilabial in Dutch (mostly) and High German, labio-dental in Norse and Low German.

    VOCALISM

    Back to Table of Contents

    34. The Indo-European Vowels

    a

    ## Make sure you get those macron/short vowels

    Until 1878 it had been assumed that Indo-European had essentially the same vowels as Aryan, namely ā ī ū, and that in the European group a split up into ā ē ō. In 1878 and 1879 several scholars showed that the five-vowel system (a e o; i u) was the original one, and that in Aryan e o had become a. The priority for this discovery belongs to Hermann Collitz.1 The Indo-European vowels are best preserved in Greek, but also fairly well in accented syllables in Latin. In Balto-Slavic and Germanic, a and o on the one hand, and ā and ō on the other 'fell together', although the treatment was by no means identical in the two groups; see 38. IE e o a may be termed the fundamental vowels. They occurred mainly in accented syllables. IE i u originally were reductions from diphthongs (39) and as such originally occurred only in unstressed syllables. Unstressed simple long vowels were reduced to a slurred vowel which may be likened to the unaccented vowel in Eng. drama. It is transcribed by ə and termed shva, a name taken from Hebrew grammar. Unstressed short vowels were generally dropped, but were sometimes preserved as slurred vowels, perhaps similar to y in Eng. pretty. This is usually transcribed by the Slavic letter ъ, (the 'soft sign'), which in present Russian merely indicates palatal articulation of the preceding consonant, but originally expressed short i. IE ə and ъ, are distinguished as 'shva primum' and 'shva secundum', but the term 'shva' alone always refers to ə.2

    Back to Table of Contents

    b

    Instances:

    Long Vowels —

    ē became ā in Sk., ī in Celtic, and was preserved in the other IE languages with the exception of Germanic (37):
    • IE dhē- -- Sk. dhā-, Gk. θη- (τίθημι) 'put', L. fecī 'I did', OSl. dēti 'put'
    • IE -—Gk. ιημι < *σισημι 'throw', L. sēmen, OIr. sīl, OSl. sēmę. 'seed'
    • IE rēĝ— Sk. rājan-, L. rēx, Gall. -rix (borrowed in Gmc. rīk- NHG Reich) 'king'.

    ō became ā in Aryan, Celtic, and Slavic (also in Lettish, but uo in Lithuanian):

    • IE dō— Sk. dānam, Gk. δωρον, L. dōnum, OSl. darъ 'gift'
    • IE ĝno— Sk. jñā-, Gk. γνω, L. (co)gnō(scō), OSl. znati 'know'
    • IE dwō (beside dwōu) — Sk. (Ved.) dvā, Gk. δυω, (L. duo, < *duō?) OIr. , OSl. dъva.

    ā became ē in Attic-Ionic Greek, ō in Lithuanian (but a in Lettish):

    • IE māter- — Sk. mātar-, Gk. Dor. ματηρ, Att. μήτηρ, L. māter, OIr. mathir, Lith. motē ('wife'), OSl. matъ 'mother'
    • IE bhrāter— Sk. bhrātar-, Gk. φρατωρ, L.frāter, OIr. brāthir, OSl. bratъ, Lith. broter(elis) 'brother'
    • IE stā- - Sk. sthā-, Gk. σταμι,στημι, L. stāre, Lith. stóti, Lett. stat, OSl. stati.

    Short Vowels —

    Back to Table of Contents

    c

    e was the most frequent IE vowei. It remained e in most languages and under most conditions, but became a in Aryan.

    • IE bhérō— - Sk. bharāmi, Gk. φέρω, L. ferō 'carry', OSl. berą
    • IE édō— Sk. admi, Gk. εδω, L. edo 'eat'
    • IE dekṃ— Sk. dáśa, Gk. δέκα, L. decem, OSl. desętъ.
    • IE ésti— Sk. ásti, Gk. εστί, L. est,

    o usually alternates with e (Ablaut, 44; cf. Gk. φέρω 'I carry', φορέω 'I usually carry, I wear'). It remains o in most languages, but appears as a in Aryan, Lithuanian (o in Lettish) and Germanic (38).

    • IE oktō(u)— Sk. aṣṭāu, Gk. οκτω, L. octo, OSl. osmъ, Lith. aštuonì
    • IE ghosti— L. hostis 'enemy', OSl. gostъ 'guest'
    • IE ghortó— Gk. χόρτος 'fenced-in place',. L. hortus 'garden', OIr. gort 'field', OSl. gradъ 'city' (from *gord-, or borrowed from Gmc.; cf. AJPh 32. 431 ff.).

    a was relatively rare. It remained a everywhere, except in Slavic, where it became o.

    • IE aĝō— Sk. ájāmi, Gk. αγω, L. agō 'lead, drive'
    • IE arō— Gk. αρόω, L. arō, Lith. ariù, OSl. orją 'plough'
    • IE nas- —Sk. nas, Lith. nasraĩ, OSl. nosъ 'nose'.

    ə is the reduction of the long vowels ē ō ā. It appears as i in Indo-Iranian, as o < a in Slavic, as a everywhere else.

    • IE pətēr- —Sk. pitár-, Gk. πατήρ, L. pater 'father' (but normal grade ā in māter, frāter)
    • IE stə- —Sk. sthitá-, Gk. στατός, L. status 'stood' (past part.), Lith. stataū 'I put', OSl. stoją 'I stand'
    • IE də— L. datus, Gk. δοτός 'given' (o for a through association with ω in δωρον 'gift', δίδωμι 'I give'; α in δάνος 'gift').

    ъ, is the partial reduction of e (46) (the full reduction is zero). It can best be attested in Greek and Latin, where it appears as a (in Gk. only before nasals and liquids) in syllables that otherwise show alter nation between e and o: pъt- (reduced form of pet- in Gk. πετάννυμι 'stretch', L. petō 'strive'): pateō 'stand open'; L. maneō 'remain', against Gk. μένω, L. magnus 'great' against Gk. μέγας; the Gk. preposition παρα 'about' by the side of περί 'around'. i u are 'zero' grades of the diphthongs ei eu (46):

    • IE bhidh-, reduced form of bheidh- (Gk. πείθω, L. fīdō)— Gk. πιθέσθαι 'trust', L. fidēs 'faith'
    • IE tri— Gk. τρι, L. tri- in compounds (full form IE trejes in Gk. τρεις, L. trēs) 'three'
    • IE kwid— Sk. cid, Gk. τί, L. quid 'what'
    • IE yugóm —Gk. ζυγόν, Sk. yugám, L. iugum 'yoke' (full form in Gk. ζεύγνυμι 'hitch')
    • IE klutós, (past part. of kleu- 'hear')— Sk. śrutás 'heard', Gk. κλυτός, L. inclutus 'famous'.

    35. The Indo-European Diphthongs

    The fundamental vowels, a e o, and also the reduced vowel e, combine with glides to form diphthongs. These glides are either i u in semi-vocalic function (written by Brugmann and many others), or homosyllabic liquids and nasals. Thus result these diphthongs: ei eu el er em en; oi ou ol or om on; ai au al etc. əi əu əl etc.; ēi ēu ēl ēr ēm ēn; ōi āi ōu āu etc. The ĕ- and ŏ- diphthongs were frequent, the others relatively rare, and it is not always possible to give reliable instances. This is partly due to the fact that in most IE languages long diphthongs either shortened the first element or lost the glide; cf. IE djēus '(God of) Heaven'—nom. sg. Sk. dyāuṣ, but Gk. Ζεύς < *Ζηυς, acc. sg. Sk. dyām, Gk. Ζην. Probably such double forms go back to Indo-European times.1

    Instances:

    I-Diphthongs: ei was contracted to ē in Sanskrit, to ī in (Classical) Latin and Old Slavic.

    • IE steigh —Gk. στείχω, L. ve-stīgium 'track', OSl. stigną 'reach'
    • IE deikō —Gk. δείκνυμι 'show', L. dīcō (early deicō) 'say'
    • IE bheidhō —Gk. πείθω 'persuade', L. fīdō 'trust'.

    oi, the alternation of ei, is ē in Sanskrit, ū in Classical Latin, generally ē in Slavic:

    • IE oinos —Gk. οινή 'one-spot', L. ūnus (early acc. sg. oino)
    • IE loikwos (from leikwō 'I leave')— Gk. λοιπός left behind', OSl otъlēkъ 'remainder'.

    ai (Sk. ē) in Sk. ēdhas 'fuel', Gk. αιθω 'shine', L. aedēs 'fire-place, house'

    əi perhaps in Gk. λαιός, L laevus, OSl. lēvъ 'left (-hand)'.

    ēi in Sk. āima, Gk. ημεν 'we went'.

    U-Diphthongs:

    eu is ou in early Latin, ū in Classical Latin, ō < au in Sanskrit, ū in Slavic:

    • IE bheudh—Gk. πεύθω, OSl b[l]ūdą, 'mind'
    • IE ĝeus— Sk. jóṣati 'he partakes', Gk. γεύομαι 'I taste'
    • IE jeug- —Gk. ζευγος 'yoke', L. iūgera 'ploughed fields'.

    ou is ō in Sanskrit, ū in Classical Latin and Old Slavic:

    • IE louk— Sk. rōcáyati 'makes shine', lōkas 'forest clearing, place', L. lūcus 'clearing, grove' (early acc. sg. loucom), OSl lūča 'ray', lučъ 'light'.
    • au or əu in Sk. ōjas 'strength', Gk. αύξάνω, L. augeō 'increase'; Gk. ταυρος 'bull', L. taurus; perhaps in L. auris, Lith. ausìs.

      eu in Sk. dyāuṣ, see above.

      ou in IE gwōus 'bovine'—Sk. gāuṣ, Gk. βους, L. bovem.

      au in IE nāus —Gk. ναυς, L. nāvem 'ship'.

    Instances for el er em en etc. are given in 39.

    Back to Table of Contents

    36. The Germanic Vowel Shift

    a

    (a) Other Indo-European Languages. In every IE language the vowel development follows more or less clearly discernible directions. Aryan shows a tendency to articulate both short and long vowels with wide mouth opening, so that ē and ō change to ā (see above). In Attic-Ionic Greek the trend is towards narrowing and fronting: a be comes ē, u becomes y (= ū). In late Latin the difference between long and short vowels disappears. Celtic, like Attic-Ionic, has a marked tendency towards vowel narrowing, cf. Gall. -rīx (Vercingetorīx) —L. rex. In Slavic, a became o, which requires more articulating energy, while ō became ā, with lessened energy, and subsequently the difference in quantity was abolished. The significance of this becomes clearly apparent by comparison with the opposite trend in Germanic.

    Back to Table of Contents

    b

    (b) The Germanic Trend.1 The most outstanding characteristic of early Germanic vowel development is the rigid adherence to the contrast between short and long vowels. As shown above, some other Indo-European languages tend to lessen or eliminate that contrast. In some, as in Aryan, the two types develop on parallel lines. But the early Germanic languages not only preserve, but in fact emphasize the contrast. This becomes clearly apparent by indicating the principal vowel trends in a diagram of the vowel articulations. The conventional 'vowel triangle' is adequate for the purpose and has the advantage of convenience; the 'vowel trapezoid' or the 'vowel ellipse'2 is more

    accurately in keeping with the physiological facts. In any case, the place of each letter indicates the place of articulation, that is, the place of highest tongue elevation for each vowel. Cf. D. Jones, Outline of English Phonetics 36. In the following diagrams arrows indicate the direction of vowel changes in Primitive Germanic and the earliest period after the formation of the several Germanic languages. Broken lines indicate conditional or restricted changes.

    From this diagram follows, as a mere, mechanical statement of fact, that long vowels and short vowels tend in opposite directions. With our arbitrary arrangement of the places of articulation (the current of breath following our direction of writing), long vowels may be said to move clockwise, short vowels, counter-clockwise.

    Physiologically speaking, in the long-vowel trend the tongue moves away from the position that it assumes when at rest. In the transition from ē to ǣ to ā the angle of the jaws is widened and the tongue correspondingly lowered and retracted. Further lowering not being possible, in the transition from ā to ō to ū the tongue is still more retracted, and necessarily raised. (The relative width of the angle of the jaws corresponds, of course, to the position of the tongue.) On the other hand, short vowels tend towards relaxation and a corresponding approach to the position of rest of the tongue. Thus, o and ə become a, and u and e change to o and i, at least under certain conditions.

    Note: The Slavic change ā > ā and a > o represents exactly the opposite tendency. Here, in the case of the short vowel the tongue moves towards a position which is further away from its Ruhelage, while it moves towards the Ruhelage in the case of the long vowel. In the first instance it is relatively tense, in the second, relatively relaxed.

    Sievers, Grundzüge der Phonetik 270: 'Kurze und lange Vokale schlagen bekanntlich bei derartigen Verschiebungen häufig entgegengesetzte Wege ein..... Hiefür liegt der Grund wohl in dem auch sonst zur Anwendung kommenden Gesetz, dass die Artikulationen eines Lautes um so energischer und sicherer vollzogen werden, je stärker derselbe sum Bewusstsein kommt, d. h. je grösser seine Stärke und Dauer ist. Dies erklärt beim langen Vokal sowohl eine Steigerung in der spezifischen Zungenartikulation (nach Stellung und Spannung) als der Rundung, falls solche vorhanden ist. Beim kurzen Vokal dagegen, der nur einen momentanen Zungenschlag erfordert, wird leicht das eigentliche Mass der Entfernung von der Ruhelage wie der Spannung nicht erreicht, d. h. es wird eine Wandlung von Vokalen mit stärkerer spezifischer Artikulation zu Lauten von mehr neutraler Artikulation angebahnt, sowohl was Zungen- und Lippenstellung als was Spannung betrifft.' This is an exact description of the Germanic vowel trend, but is not in keeping with most other IE languages, especially Slavic.

    This statement of the physiological fact does not explain the Germanic vowel shift, but merely describes it. Another factor should not be lost sight of: Acoustically, long vowels move in the direction of lower pitch, short vowels, of higher pitch (Eigenton). It is difficult to decide whether this movement in the scale i-e-a-o-u was purely accidental or was one of the contributing causes. At any rate, the Germanic vowel shift increases the articulatory contrasts between the two series of vowels, a fact that agrees with the Germanic consonant shift and several aspects of Germanic morphology.

    Back to Table of Contents

    37. The Germanic Long Vowels

    Two changes are generally ascribed to Primitive Germanic: that of ā to ō and of ē to ǣ. The latter is largely a 'Phonetic Interpolation', i.e., the assumption of a transitional stage between ē and ā. In Gothic, ǣ reverts to ē by a different process of tensing (raising and fronting); in North and West Germanic, a results; this develops secondarily into ǣ in Old English, and ē in Frisian.

    Old English ǣ was further narrowed to [e:] in Middle English, and to [i:] in New English: dǣd > [de:d] > [di: d]. Gmc. ō became [u:] both in New English and New High German. OHG, MHG uo is a transitional stage: OE dōm > NE [du:m] - NHG --tum.

    • IE ē: dhē- (Gk. τίθημι , etc., see 34)— Go. gadēþs, OE dǣd, OFris. dēd, OS dād, OHG tāt 'deed'
    • IE sē- (L. sēmen, etc.) —Go. mana-sēþs 'mankind', ON sād, OE sǣd, OFris. sēd, OS sād, OHG sātM 'seed'
    • IE ēd- (L. ēdimus)—Go. ētum, OE ǣton, OFris. *ēton, OS *ātun, OHG āzum 'we ate'.

    ō: Usually in Ablaut with ē.

    • IE bhlō- (L. flōs)— Go. blōma, ON blōme, OE blōma, OS blōmo, OHG bluomo 'bloom, flower'
    • IE dhōm- (Gk. θωμός 'heap')— Go. dōms, ON dōmr, OE OS dōm, OHG tuom 'sentence (setting-down)'
    • IE plō- (Gk. πλωτός 'floating')— Go. flōdus, ON flōþ, OE OS flōd, OHG fluot 'flood'.
    ā was rare; it does not show any ablaut relations, except with its weakened grade ə:

    • IE māter: (L. māter, etc.) —Go. *mōdar (Wulfila uses only Go aiþeimōþer, OE mōdor, OFris. mōder, OS mōdar, OHG muoter
    • IE bhrāter- (L. frāter, etc.) —Go. brōþar (Crimean Go. bruder), ON brōþer OE brōþor, OFris. brōther, OS brōthar, OHG bruoder
    • IE stā- (L. stāre, etc.)— Go. ON stōþ, OE OFris. OS stōd, OHG stuo(n)t 'stood'.

    ī ū were very rare. They remain unchanged:
  • Back to Table of Contents

    38. The Germanic Short Vowels

    a

    (1) The changes o > a and 9 > o are 'unconditional', that is, they are independent of surrounding sounds.

    (2) The changes e >i and u > ə are 'conditional'. The drift is inhibited by an incompatible vowel in the following syllable, but favored by a compatible vowel: e remains before a mid- or low-vowel (usually Gmc. a), and u remains before the high vowels i and u. But the influence of the following vowel was counteracted by an intervening nasal group ('nx' = m, n + any consonant), in the Anglo-Frisian group even by a nasal alone. This is due to the fact that a nasal group forms a more effective syllable barrier than other consonants or consonant groups. The result is the following formula (cf. 42 a):

    IE e > Gmc. i, but e + a remains e; enx changes always to inx (*pelu 'much' > OHG filu, *kelono- 'conceal' > OHG helan, but *wentos 'wind' > wint)

    IE u > Gmc. o, but u + i, u remains u; unx remains always (*jugom 'yoke' > OHG joh, but *kˆmtóm '100' > OHG hund-).

    Back to Table of Contents

    b

    Notes: 1. The conventional formula is virtually the reverse: e > i, (a) before i j, perhaps also u (at least in OHG)

    (b) before nasal plus consonant

    (c) when unaccented.

    The result is nearly the same, but the new formulation is a clearer expression of the physiological trend. Also, it is more easily reconciled with the apparently irregular treatment of e before u. In OHG, e + u > i is carried through consistently, but in Norse and Anglo-Frisian this change is easily prevented by leveling of forms or by vowel-lowering consonants: L. pecu—OHG fihu, but ON , OE feoh 'cattle'; IE pelu- (Gk. πολύ)—Go. filu, OHG filu, but ON fiǫl < *felu, OE feolu (OE 'breaking', see 42) 'much'.

    It is phonetically improbable that the influence of a following u actually changed e to i. It merely did not exert that restraining influence that we find in the case of a following a.

    2. In Gothic, u and e are independent of following vowels: u remained u, and e became i everywhere (except before h, r; 42). 3. Gmc. a in closed syllables became, in general, s» in OE, e in OFris. : OE blaed, OFris. bled-OHG blat 'leaf. 4. The change a > e is treated in 41.

    Instances:

    Back to Table of Contents

    c

    IE e > Gmc. i, unless followed by an IE mid- or low vowel, i.e., ē ō ā. Leveling has to an extent obscured the original conditions. As far as expedient, such leveled forms are bracketed in the following lists.

    (Verb Endings: Infinitive, IE -onom > Gmc. -an; sg. ind. pres., IE > Go. -a, NWGmc. -u, which disappears in ON and appears as -e in OE (West Saxon).)

    IE o > Gmc. a:

    IE a = Gmc. a:

    IE ə > Gmc. a:

    Back to Table of Contents

    d

    IE ъ, (shva secundum) appears in Germanic as u at least before and after nasals and liquids, probably as e elsewhere; cf. Hirt, HU 1. 62 f.; Guntert, Idg. Ablautprobleme 31.

    According to Sievers, Btr. 16. 236, Streitberg, IF Ans. 2. 48, also shva primum could appear as u in Germanic, especially in unaccented syllables, cf. Sk. vavṛ-tima : Go. waurþum. The matter is still a moot question. The most probable instance is the Go. preposition du, reduced grade of *dē, , which can hardly be explained in any other way. Also y in OE dyde 'did' is perhaps u < ə, with umlaut; but cf. 75 b.

    ъ, seems to have become u in syllables that were unstressed or weakly stressed in Germanic: OHG -zug '-ty' over against Go. tigjus; L. anas, OHG anut (and enit) 'duck'; Gk. γέρανος, OHG kranuh 'crane'.

    IE ĝhъmōn (different ablaut grade in OL. hemo; nēmo < *ne-hemo; Class. L. homō)— Go., OE guma, ON gume, OFris. OS OHG gomo 'man'

    IE tъl- (L. tollo)—Go. þulan, ON þola, OE þolian, OFris. tholia, OS tholian, OHG dolēn 'suffer'

    IE sъd- (full form in L. sedeō, Go. sitan, etc.) —Go. sitans, ON setenn, OE seten, OS gisetan, OHG gisezzan 'sat'.

    IE i (reduced grade of ei/ot):

    IE bhidh- (L. fidēs, etc.)— Go. bidjan, ON biþia, OE biddan, OFris. bidda, OS biddian, OHG bitten 'ask'

    IE widmén 'we know' (Gk. ιδμεν) —Go. witum, ON vitom, OE witon, OS witum, OHG wizzum.

    IE u (reduced grade of eu/ou):

    IE bhudh- (Gk. πυνθάνομαι, full grade in πεύθομαι 'find out') —Go. -budum, ON buþom, OE budon, OS bundun, OHG butun 'we offered, ordered'

    IE jugom (Gk. ζυγόν, L. iugum)—Go. juk, ON ok, OE geoc, OHG joch 'yoke'

    Back to Table of Contents

    e

    Chronology. The exact time of the Germanic vowel shift can hardly be ascertained, but there are indications that point to the period shortly before and after the beginning of the Christian Era. Germanic loan words in Finnish as well as the earliest Latin renderings of Germanic names have a for IE o: Finnish ansas 'log' — Go. ans, L. onus 'burden' (?), Langobardi, related to L. longus; the latter word, like Ariovistus, Chariovaldus, etc., seems to indicate that unaccented o in compounds retained its quality longer than accented o (cf. Streitberg, UG §55), but it is more likely that such forms are due to the influence of similar Latin compounds, such as Ahenobarbus, which, in turn, may have been imita tions of the Greek type ροδοδάκτυλος. The shift ā > ō and ē > ǣ, ā seems to belong to the Christian Era; Caesar has Silva Bācenis (related to φαγός, fagus) as against OHG Buochunna 'beech forest' — Bohemian Forest, and Celtic names that were most probably adopted during the first century A.D. show Germanic ō: Dānuvius, Go. Dōnawi. Cf. Streitberg, UG §59. Gmc. e for later i is preserved during the first century A.D.: Tacitus has Segimundus, Venedi; Streitberg, UG §66. Gmc ē is found in Caesar's Suēvi for later Schwaben, Streitberg, UG §77. Cf. Bremer Relative Sprachchronologie, IF 4.8 B. ff., and Trautmann, Germanische Lautgesetze.

    39

    Back to Table of Contents

    a

    The Germanic Diphthongs with i,- or u-glides tend to become monophthongs through contraction. See 40. Aside from this, the regular change of o ə > a, e > i, in part also that of u > o, takes place. Thus, oi əi ou əu appear as ai au, like ei > ii = ī; eu appears normally as eo before a, as iu elsewhere.

    In the 'nil grade' (46), the first element is lost, so that i and u appear as genuine vowels. The nasal and liquid glides (13) develop in Germanic the vowel u, which is treated like u as the nil grade of an u-diphthong (u/a > o, 41). Therefore, we find the following alternations: ī—ai—i; iu/eo—au—u; el/il—al—ul/ol; er/ir—ar—ur/or; em/im—am —um/om; en/in—an—un/on.

    In the following instances, monophthongized forms except IE ei = Gmc. i are given in parentheses. For Go. ei ai au see 40 a and 42 c.

    IE steigh-: stiogh-: stigh- climb, go (Gk. στείχω)
    Go. steigan staig stigum stigans
    ON stīga (stē) stigom stigenn
    OE stīgan (stāh) stigon stigenn
    OS stīgan (stēg) stigun gistigan
    OHG stīgan steig stigum gistigan
    IE bheudh-: bhoudh-: bhudh- bid (Gk. πεύθομαι)
    Go. =-biudan -biudis bauþ budum budans
    ON biōþa (FN1) bȳþr (FN1) bauþ buþom boþenn
    OE bēodan (FN2) bīedst (FN2) bēad budon boden
    OS biodan (FN3) biudis (FN2) (bōd) budun gibodan
    OHG biotan (FN4) biutis (FN4) (bōt) butum gibotan
    IE wert-: wort-: wrt- (L. vertō)
    Go. wairþan warþ waurþum waurþans
    ON verþa varþ urþom orþenn
    OE weorþan wearþ wurdon worden
    OS werthan warth wurdun giwordan
    OHG werdan ward wurtum giwortan
    IE bhendh-: bhondh-: bhndh- (Gk. πεισμα < *φενθ-σμα 'band')
    Go. bindan band bundum bundans
    ON binda batt bundom bundenn
    OE bindan bond bundon bundenn
    OS bindan band bundun gibundan
    OHG bintan bant buntum gibuntan
    FN1 41d. FN2 41f. 42k. FN3 42k. FN4 42l.

    Back to Table of Contents

    b

    Long Diphthongs, with the exception of ēi, cannot be recognized as such from Germanic evidence, since they either shortened their first element or, more rarely, lost the second element:

    IE ōi > Gmc. ai in ON fleire 'more', L. ploirume 'most', Sk. prāya- 'more'

    IE āi > Gmc. ai in Go. aiws 'time', L. aevum, Sk. āyuṣ 'life, time'

    IE ēu > Gmc. eu in OHG Ziu, ON Tȳr, Gk. Ζεύς, Sk. dyāuṣ

    IE āu > Gmc. au in ON naust 'boat shed', L. nāvis, Sk. nāuṣ 'ship'

    IE ōu > Gmc. au in ON tuau 'two', Sk. dvāu

    IE ēn > Gmc. en (in) in Go. winds, L. ventus < IE wē-n-tos (Sk. vāyati 'blows').

    Loss of the second element occurs, e.g., in: Go. flōdus 'flood', ON flōþ, OE OS OFris. flōd, OHG fluot, Gk. πλωτός 'floating', alternating with IE pleu/plou- in Gk. πλέϝω 'float', Sk. plávatē 'swims', ON fliōta, OE flēotan, OS fliotan, OHG fliozan 'flow'. It is hardly possible in instances of this kind to decide whether we have to deal with original long-diphthongs with shortening of the first element, or with a lengthening of original short-diphthongs.

    IE ēi seems to have lost its second element in every case; the resulting Gmc. ē was narrow and is generally expressed by ē, in contrast to IE ē > Gmc ǣ. In OHG this ē was further narrowed to ia ie. In Gothic, IE ē and ēi appear as ē. Reliable instances are rare; IE lengthened grade of k^ei- (Gk. ε-κει, OS hīr show the normal grade; reduced grade in L. cis, citra, Go. himma, hina, hita) gave Go. ON OE hēr, OHG hiar. IE stēigh-, lengthened grade of steigh- in Goth. steigan, etc., gave OHG stiaga 'Stiege, stair'. IE skēi-ro- (skei- 'shine') yielded OHG skiari 'clever' (normal grade in Go. skeirs, ON skīrr, OE scīr, OS skīr 'clear', Go. skeinan 'shine').

    For the large literature on this moot question cf. Streitberg, UG 65; Hirt, HU 1. 33.

    Brugmann, IF 6. 89 ff., and Wood, Germanic Studies, assume IE ēi as normal grade of an original long (not lengthened) IE diphthong in the Norse and West Germanic preterites of the i-group of those strong verbs that are reduplicating in Gothic, such as ON OE OS hēt, OHG hiaz, cf. 54

    In addition to this problematic diphthongal origin of ē, narrow Gmc. ē appears in West Germanic as compensatory lengthening for the loss of a following z. similar to L. nīdus < *ni-zdos, as in OE mēd, OS mēda, OHG miata 'wages': Go. mizdō, Gk. μισυός.

    Finally, it generally represents ē in loan-words from Latin: OHG ziagal < (late) L. tēgula 'tile', briaf < late L. brēve 'letter', Kriach < Graecus (pron. [gre:kus]).

    Back to Table of Contents

    40. Monophthongization and Diphthongization.

    a

    (a) Monophthongization. Due to the strong stress accent of Germanic syllables, the first elementof diphthongs tended to absorb the semi-vowel glide, so that ei ai au easily became monophthongs, ī ā/ē ā/ā. Gmc. eu/iu resisted this trend longer; being a combination of a front and a back vowel, its component elements were more strongly contrasted.

    IE ei became Gmc. ī everywhere in prehistoric times. The Gothicspelling ei is taken over from the Greek spelling of Wulfila's time, in which ει represented [i:]: IE steigh-: Go. steigan, ON stīga, OE OS OHG stīgan.

    Gmc. ai au more or less preserved their diphthongal character into historical times, in part even to the present. For Gothic, the standard view assumes that the spellings ai au in Wulfila's writing indicated diphthongs where this is indicated by the etymology, but monophthongs [ɛ ɔ],where they stand for i u before h r (42i). Jacob Grimm introduced the device of spelling ái áu for diphthongal, aí aú for monophthongal function: wáit [wait] = Gk. οιδα 'I know', áuso [auso:] = L. auris 'ear', but baíra [bɛra] = L. ferō 'bear', daúr [dor] = Gk. θύρα door'. Hirt, HU1. 39f. presents weighty arguments for the monophthongal character of Wulfila's ai au regardless of their origin; of course, there must at least have been a difference in quantity, short vowels representing the original monophthongs, long vowels, [ɛ: ɔ:] the original diphthongs; Wulfila's e o surely denoted the closed vowels [e: o:]. (To Hirt's examples might be added Jordanes' Oium < *akwjom, *aȝwja-,*auja- name of a watery place, and Berig = Boiorīx, *Bajarīks.) I believe that Hirt is right, but the question is largely one of chronology. Shortly before Wulfila's time, Gmc. ai au were still diphthongs in Gothic, as is indicated by the spelling (Gutan(e) Iowi) hailag (R. Loewe's interpretation) on the gold ring of Bukarest and spellings in Latin and Greek historians, like Radagaisus, Austrogothi (cf. Streitberg, UG 58 ff.). Shortly after Wulfila, they were monophthongs (Ostrogothi). Also inclosely related Vandalic, ai au had become [ɛ: ɔ:]: froja, armes 'domine, miserere' = Go. frauja, armais (Streitberg, UG 38). Crimean Gothic has Broe(t) 'panis', Oeghene 'oculi' = Go. *brauþ, augōna, where oe doubtless stands for [o:] or [ɔ:]; geen may possibly correspond to atheoretical Go. *gaian. In spite of the probability of Hirt's view, it seems expedient to retain the distinction between diphthongal and monophthongal pronunciation in grammatical discussions: baíran, taíhun, baúrans, taúhans = [bɛran, teɧun, bɔrans, tɔɧans], but áirus, taih, áugo, táuh = [airus, taih, auȝo:, tauh].

    Go. iu < Gmc. eu/iu was a diphthong.

    In Old Norse, the Germanic diphthongs in general remaineddiphthongal (but cf. 42 j) : ON veit, steinn, auga, þiūfr, giōta (41 e) = Go. wait, stains, augo, þiufs, giutan. In modern Norse, they are monophthongs: Norw. vet, sten, øie, tyv (Sw. tjuf = [ty:f]), gyte.

    In Old English, ai had become ā before the time of our earliest documents: stān, ān = Go. stains, ains. Gmc. au eu/iu remained diphthongal until the beginning of the Middle English period; au is spelled ēa = [æə], eu is expressed by ēo = [iə] (cēosan), iu usually by īo = [iə] (līode — OHG liuti 'people'). The macron on the first element,relatively rare in MSS, but used consistently in our editions and grammars, is merely an orthographical device to distinguish these diphthongs from 'broken vowels' (42 e). The spelling ea for æa is due to the scribal tradition mentioned in 24 —In ME monophthongization continued, so that both Gmc: au and Gmc. eu finally resulted in [i:]: OE dēop, hēap— [di:p, hi:p]. In part, this was deflected by secondary changes: OE dēad, lēoht > [dɛd, lait].

    In Old Frisian, ai became ē or ā, and au became ā; in Old Saxon, ai appears as ē, au as ō: OFris. wēt, stēn, ā < *aiwa- 'right', āgun —Go. aigun 'they have', āge 'eye'; OS wēt, stēn, ōga. Gmc. eu remained a diphthong in both dialects.

    Old High German ordinarily preserved the diphthongal character of ai au eu: weiz, stein, ouga; but before certain consonants ai au became ē ō. Cf. 42 l.

    Toward the end of the Middle Ages, OHG ie < Gmc. ē² and uo < Gmc. ō were monophthongized in Middle German, and this was adopted in Standard German while the diphthongs still largely prevail in Upper German: OHG briaf, brief > [bri:f], guot > [gu:t]. Likewise, the mutated vowel of uo, üe, became [y:]: MHG > [gy:tə].

    Back to Table of Contents

    b>

    (b) DIPHTHONGIZATION

    Although apparently the opposite of monophthongization, diphthongizationin Germanic languages was also a consequence of thestrong stress accent. Long vowels tended to increase the energy of their articulation. This had led to the Germanic vowel shift described in 37 and to the later English raising of ē ō to [i: u:]. In Old High German, a similar process started during the eighth century, when ē² became ea ia ie, and ō became oa ua uo: briaf, guot. These diphthongs mark the transition to Middle German [i: u:] (see above).

    A more extensive diphthongization took place in English, Dutch, and most of High German before the end of the Middle Ages. The high vowels[i: u: y:] were incapable of further raising. The stress accent seems to have caused further lengthening, resulting in 'slurred' accent (rising-falling accent, circumflex accent, 'Schleifton') and finally in diphthongization. During the articulation of the over-long vowel the tongue reached the highest point only gradually; thus, there developed a glide from a mid-vowel, later even a low vowel, to a high vowel,[i: > ei ai, u: > au]. The new diphthongal element was gradually more or less assimilated to the first part of the diphthong, so that now-a-days English and German generally have the diphthongs [ae ao]: OE OHG mīn, hūs, NE NHG [maen, haos], MHG = [ly:tə], NHG Leute = [lɔytə, lɔøtə].

    Back to Table of Contents

    41

    a

    Mutation ('Umlaut',a term coined by Jacob Grimm) in the widest sense can be defined as the modification of a vowel under the influence of and in the direction of the articulation of a neighboring vowel. In the Germanic languages the use of the term is essentially restricted to the change of a stem vowel in the direction of the vowel of a suffix or ending. Similar processes are found in many languages. In Russian, a is a low-back vowel before a back vowel, but a low-front vowel. before a front vowel: dar(ъ) 'gift' [dar] < *daro- , but danъ 'tribute' [dæñ] < *dani-. Irish shows a similar trend. In these cases the phonetic process is fairly clear. The intervening consonant is assimilated to the articulation of the following vowel, and the preceding vowel is assimilated to the consonant. This is a corollary to the peculiar plasticity of these languages. In some of the Ugro-Finnic languages there exists a similar process termed 'Vowel Harmony'. But there the stem vowel determines the articulation of the suffix vowel, requiring front or back articulation respectively, in accordance with its own type : Finn. huva 'good', pähä 'bad'. In a somewhat mechanical way we may designate the former process as physiological, and the latter as psychological assimilation. The impression of the position of the tongue is retained through the following syllable, regardless of whether the tongue is raised in front or in back.

    Germanic mutation, while not identical with either of these two types of mutation, bears a certain relationship to both of them. Its origin is not known, but the following may be an approach to an interpretation of the process. The conditional changes e > i, u > o in the Germanic Vowel Shift (38) were soon followed by the corresponding conditional change a > e in Norse and West Germanic. But while the former two changes were merely prevented by a following incompatible vowel, and therefore occurred also in final syllables, the change a > e depended on a following i (j). These three mutations tended in the direction of the general vowel drift. They implied an enrichment of the morphological pattern of the language. Distinctions between forms by gradation (45) had been inherited from the Indo-European parent speech. The contrast between OHG geban and gab was the continuation of the Indo-European alternation between e and o. In consequence of mutation, a new type of interchange between stem vowels developed, e.g., OHG beran: biris, hulfun: gifolfan, faran: feris. In some of the Germanic languages this became a grammatical device of great importance. In connection with the weakening of final syllables,which had started before the period of mutation (49, A 1), it assumed functional value as an auxiliary means of distinction between singular and plural, indicative and optative, positive and comparative, and other morphological categories. Perhaps this was one of the factors that led to a great extension of the new linguistic device, which no longer followed the general direction of the vowel drift. So far as the endings themselves are concerned, the speech feeling for the phonetic law back of them gradually disappeared. It is obvious, for instance, that in Old English there could not exist any recollection of the fact that the difference between ō in fōt 'foot' and ē in fēt 'feet' was due to the former plural ending -iz. All that remained was the functional variation of the stem vowel, which differed in degree in the various Germanic languages. It found its greatest development in High German, particularly in the formation of noun plurals. Through the weakening of endings, Old High German possessed many plural forms that did not differ from the corresponding singulars, e.g. wort, turi, bruoder, man, naht. In such forms, mutation was introduced during the Middle High German and New High German periods, in imitation of such clear contrasts as gastgesti (analogical mutation), so that the number of mutated plurals in standard New High German is approximately six times as great as in Old High German.1 A particularly significant feature of this analogical process is the development of [ø]. Phonologically, this sound could not have developed at all. Short o, from which it is nominally derived, could exist only as a-mutation of Gmc. u;but the change o > ø as a phonetic transition, would require i, not a, in the following syllable. Therefore, an alternation of suffixes with a and i would imply an alternation of the stem vowels o and y, not o and ø. We have certain instances of this alternation, e.g., ON holpenn : hylpe — NHG geholfen: hülfe; OE OHG gold < *guldo-: OE gylden, NHG gülden. But in most cases this alternation is leveled out (NE NHG golden; NHG hälfe, hölfe).

    In the most extreme form of mutation, which we find in Old Norse, we may say that every back vowel was fronted by a following front vowel, every front vowel rounded by a following back (rounded) vowel.

    Thus, the following fundamental system results:

    Before i (j): Before u (w):
    ă > ǣ e

    ă > ŏ
    ŏ > ø ĕ > ø
    ŭ > ȳ˘ ĭ > ȳ˘

    Note: In a limited way, a caused mutation of i to e, contrary to the direction of the drift. This seems to indicate that Gmc. i and e were practically one 'phoneme'; before i, both vowels appeared as i, before a, as e. Cf. 36. IE wiros 'man' > OHG wer; IE stighos > OHG steg; IE ni-zdom > OHG nest. But this change occurred only in comparatively isolated forms. Within the system of the strong verb it was counteracted by the leveling influence of other forms. Thus we find OHG gestigan, without a-mutation, under the combined influence of stigun and stīgan. But the change of u to o, which is in the direction of the drift, resists such leveling: gigozzan, in spite of the form guzzun.

    Back to Table of Contents

    b

    Mutation on this large scale began perhaps in the fifth or sixth bcentury and therefore does not exist in Wulfila's Gothic. Visi-Gothic names (Ega, Egica, Egila, Emila, cf. Streitberg, UG 2. 78) seem to indicate mutation of a to e. Crimean Gothic does not happen to offer any clear instances. In Norse and West Germanic, this peculiar fact appears, which I am unable to interpret: The extent of consonant shift stands in inverse ratio to the extent (and, probably, time) of mutation, i.e., the more consonant shift any given dialect shows, the more restricted is the scope of mutation in that dialect. This implies that Old Norse has the widest, Bavarian the narrowest scope of mutation, while the intermediate dialects are graded in a most consistent way in accordance with this principle.

    The following outline does not attempt to go into details, but merely indicates the general trend.

    Back to Table of Contents

    c

    In Gothic, in addition to the factor of chronology, the narrowing of e to i and of ǣ to ē were unfavorable to the spread of mutation, and the widening influence of h and r also counteracted a change that was contingent, not on the following consonant, but on the vowel of the following syllable. Go. bairis [bɛris]: bairan, nimis: niman do not admit any distinction between Gmc. e and i.

    Back to Table of Contents

    d

    Norse developed vowel mutation very extensively, virtually to the limit of the general scheme indicated above. The earliest Runic inscriptions (4th to 6th century) show some unmutated forms: gastiʀ > gestr.2 The following instances illustrate its chief features:

    i-mutation:

    • a > e, *ȝastiz > gestr
    • ā > ǣ, lāta 'let', 2 sg., lǣtr < *latiz
    • o > ø, Run. dohtriʀ > *døhtr > døtr
    • ō > ø, Go. dōmjan 'judge', ON døma
    • u > y, Go. þugkjan 'seem', ON þykkia
    • ū > ȳ, *mūsiz 'mice', ON mȳss
    • au > ey, Go. hlaupis 'you run', ON hleypr
    • iu > ȳ, Go. *þliugis 'you fly', ON flygr.
    u-mutation:

    • a > ǫ, Go. magus 'son', ON mǫgr; Go. aƕa 'water', ON *ahwu, *ahu > ǫ 'river'
    • e > ø, Go. riqis < *rekwes (Gk. ερεβος), ON røkkr
    • i > y, Go. triggws 'faithful', ON tryggr; OE Tīu (L. dīvus), name of a god, ON Tȳr.

    Back to Table of Contents

    e

    'Breaking' in Old Norse can be defined as incomplete mutation. Genuine mutationiscaused by the high vowels i u. Breaking is caused chiefly by a, less often by u; e before a becomes ia,e before u becomes io:
    • OHG herza, ON hiarta 'heart'
    • OHG bergan, ON biarga 'conceal'
    • OHG erda, ON *erþu > iorþ 'earth'
    • Go. hairns, ON hiorr 'sword'.

    This leads to considerable variations in paradigms, e.g., skioldr 'shield' < *skelduz, gen. sg. skialdar < *skeldauz, nom. pl. skilder < *skeldiuz.